Revision as of 09:47, 30 September 2009 view sourceDistributivejustice (talk | contribs)352 editsm cite doi← Previous edit | Latest revision as of 06:37, 5 January 2025 view source Citation bot (talk | contribs)Bots5,447,908 edits Added work. | Use this bot. Report bugs. | Suggested by Eastmain | #UCB_webform 307/454 | ||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
{{short description|Discussions and claims of differences in intelligence along racial lines}} | |||
{{dablink|This article also discusses issues regarding ethnicity and intelligence.}} | |||
{{pp-vandalism|small=yes}} | |||
{{POV|date=May 2009}} | |||
{{ |
{{pp|small=yes}} | ||
<noinclude>{{Race}}</noinclude> | |||
{{Unbalanced|date=April 2008}} | |||
{{Expert-subject|Psychology|date=November 2008}} | |||
{{Race}} | |||
Discussions of '''race and intelligence''' – specifically regarding claims of differences in intelligence along racial lines – have appeared in both ] and ] since the modern concept of ] was first introduced. With the inception of ] in the early 20th century, differences in average test performance between racial groups have been observed, though these differences have fluctuated and in many cases steadily decreased over time. Complicating the issue, modern science has concluded that race is a ] phenomenon rather than a biological reality, and there exist various conflicting definitions of ]. In particular, the ] as a metric for ] is disputed. Today, the scientific consensus is that ] does not explain differences in IQ test performance between groups, and that observed differences are environmental in origin. | |||
While each is individually contentious, the topics of '''race and intelligence''' when considered together have led to ongoing and heated debate.<ref>Steven Rose. Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? NO: Science and society do not benefit Nature 457, 786-788 (12 February 2009) {{Cite doi|10.1038/457786a}}</ref><ref>Stephen Ceci and Wendy M. Williams. Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? YES: The scientific truth must be pursued. Nature 457, 788-789 (12 February 2009) {{Cite doi|10.1038/457788a}}</ref><ref></ref><ref></ref><ref></ref><ref>http://www.genpaku.org/skepticj/iqrace.html</ref><ref name="Intelligence, Race, and Genetics"> American Psychologist. Vol 60(1), Jan 2005, 46-59.</ref> This debate focuses on the nature, causes, and rectifications of ] differences in intelligence test scores. Fundamental to understanding the debate is the question, "What are the relative roles of ] in causing individual and group differences in cognitive ability?"<ref name="30yrs240">Rushton J. Philippe and Jensen Arthur R. ''Psychology, Public Policy, and Law'' 2005, Vol. 11, No. 2, 235–294 DOI: 10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235</ref> While research and debate on race and intelligence encompasses a variety of topics, the nature-nurture question attracts the most public attention. The only consistent claim of consensus on this issue is that as of yet no one knows what causes racial group difference in cognitive ability because no single answer is widely supported<ref>Hunt. Book review. Intelligence (2009) pp. 1-2</ref>. | |||
] claims of inherent differences in intelligence between races have played a central role in the history of ]. The first tests showing differences in IQ scores between different population groups in the United States were the tests of ] recruits in ]. In the 1920s, groups of ] lobbyists argued that these results demonstrated that ] and certain immigrant groups were of inferior intellect to ] ], and that this was due to innate biological differences. In turn, they used such beliefs to justify policies of ]. However, other studies soon appeared, contesting these conclusions and arguing that the Army tests had not adequately controlled for environmental factors, such as socioeconomic and educational ]. | |||
Controversy centers on the results of ] studies conducted both in the industrialized nations such as ] and ] as well as developing nations.<ref> ''Innovations Report'' 2005-04-26</ref> Controversies endure, too, over the nature of '']'', the purpose of '']'', whether it can be quantified, whether the '']'' (IQ) is a culturally biased measure, and whether intelligence is culturally malleable. | |||
Later observations of phenomena such as the ] and disparities in access to ] highlighted ways in which environmental factors affect group IQ differences. In recent decades, as understanding of ] has advanced, claims of inherent differences in intelligence between races have been broadly rejected by scientists on both ] and ] grounds. | |||
== History == | |||
The opinion that there are differences in the brain structures or sizes of different racial and ethnic groups was widely held and studied during the 19th and early 20th centuries.<ref>Broca 1873; Bean 1906; Mall 1909; Morton 1839; Pearl 1934; Vint 1934</ref> During this time period, research on race and intelligence was often used to claim that one race was superior to another, justifying the poor status and treatment of the "inferior" race.<ref>''], ], and the Metaphysics of Race'' Rutledge M. Dennis The Journal of Negro Education, Vol. 64, No. 3, Myths and Realities: African Americans and the Measurement of Human Abilities (Summer, 1995), pp. 243–252</ref> | |||
== History of the controversy == | |||
Sir ], a ] and ] (1822–1911), spurred interest in the study of mental abilities, particularly as they relate to ] and ].<ref></ref> Galton claimed from his field observations in Africa that the African people were significantly below Anglo-Saxons' position in the normal frequency distribution of general mental ability; these claims continue to spark controversy in academia.<ref></ref> | |||
{{Main|History of the race and intelligence controversy}} | |||
{{See also|Scientific racism}} | |||
] and abolitionist ] (1817–1895) served as a high-profile counterexample to myths of black intellectual inferiority.]] | |||
Claims of differences in intelligence between races have been used to justify ], ], ], ], and racial ]s. Claims of intellectual inferiority were used to justify British wars and colonial campaigns in Asia.<ref name="Mercer-2023">{{Cite web |last=Mercer |first=Jonathan |date=October 1, 2023 |title=Racism, Stereotypes, and War |url=https://direct.mit.edu/isec/article/48/2/7/118111/Racism-Stereotypes-and-War |access-date=2024-02-04 |website=direct.mit.edu |publisher=Journal of International Security}}</ref> Racial thinkers such as ] in France relied crucially on the assumption that black people were innately inferior to white people in developing their ideologies of ]. Even ] thinkers such as ], a slave owner, believed black people to be innately inferior to white people in physique and intellect.{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|p=23}} At the same time in the United States, prominent examples of African-American genius such the ] and abolitionist ], the pioneering sociologist ], and the poet ] stood as high-profile counterexamples to widespread stereotypes of black intellectual inferiority.<ref name="LawsonKirkland1999">Stewart, Roderick M. 1999. "The Claims of Frederick Douglass Philosophically Considered." Pp. 155–56 in ''Frederick Douglass: A Critical Reader'', edited by B. E. Lawson and F. M. Kirkland. Wiley-Blackwell. {{ISBN|978-0-631-20578-4}}. "Moreover, though he does not make the point explicitly, again the very fact that Douglass is ably disputing this argument on this occasion celebrating a select few's intellect and will (or moral character)—this fact constitutes a living counterexample to the narrowness of the pro-slavery definition of humans."</ref><ref>Marable, Manning (2011), ''Living Black History: How Reimagining the African-American Past Can Remake America's Racial Future'', p. 96. {{ISBN|978-0-465-04395-8}}.</ref> In Britain, Japan's military victory over Russia in the ]<ref name="Mercer-2023" /> began to reverse negative stereotypes of "oriental" inferiority.<ref name="Tonooka-2017">{{Cite journal |last=Tonooka |first=Chika |date=2017 |title=Reverse Emulation and the Cult of Japanese Efficiency in Edwardian Britain |url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/26343378 |journal=The Historical Journal |volume=60 |issue=1 |pages=95–119 |doi=10.1017/S0018246X15000539 |jstor=26343378 |s2cid=162698331 |issn=0018-246X}}</ref> ] (1857–1911), inventor of the first intelligence test|alt=|left]] | |||
=== Early IQ testing === | |||
The scientific debate on the contribution of ] to individual and group differences in intelligence can be traced back to at least the mid-19th century.<ref>Degler 1992; Loehlin et al. 1975</ref> Beginning in the 1930s, race difference research and ] — the belief that ] are the primary cause of differences in intelligence among human groups — began to fall out of favor in psychology and anthropology after major internal debates.<ref>According to historian of psychology Graham Richards there was widespread critical debate within psychology about the conceptual underpinnings of this early race difference research (Richards 1997). These include Estabrooks (1928) two papers on the limitations of methodology used in the research; Dearborn and Long’s (1934) overview of the criticisms by several psychologists (Garth, Thompson, Peterson, Pinter, Herskovits, Daniel, Price, Wilkerson, Freeman, Rosenthal and C.E. Smith) in a collection they edited and Klineburg, who wrote three major critiques, one in 1928, and two in 1935. Richards also notes that with over a 1000 publications within psychology during the interwar years there had been a large internal debate. Towards the end of the time period almost all those publishing, including most of those who began with a pro-race differences stance, were firmly arguing against race differences research. Richards regards the scientific controversy to be dead at this point, although he also suggests reasons for its re-emergence in the late nineteen sixties.</ref> By 1961, the mainstream view was that there were no race differences in intelligence, or if there were, they were solely the result of environmental factors.<ref>Lynn 2001 pp. 67–69</ref> | |||
The first practical intelligence test, the ], was developed between 1905 and 1908 by ] and ] in France for school placement of children. Binet warned that results from his test should not be assumed to measure innate intelligence or used to label individuals permanently.{{sfn|Plotnik|Kouyoumdjian|2011}} Binet's test was translated into English and revised in 1916 by ] (who introduced IQ scoring for the test results) and published under the name ]. In 1916 Terman wrote that Mexican-Americans, African-Americans, and Native Americans have a mental "dullness seems to be racial, or at least inherent in the family stocks from which they come."<ref>{{cite book |last=Terman |first=Lewis |title=The Measurement Of Intelligence |publisher=Houghton, Mifflin and Company |year=1916 |page=91 |oclc=557712625}}</ref> | |||
The US Army used a different set of tests developed by ] to evaluate draftees for World War I. Based on the Army's data, prominent psychologists and eugenicists such as ], ], and Princeton professor ] wrote that people from southern and eastern Europe were less intelligent than native-born Americans or immigrants from the Nordic countries, and that black Americans were less intelligent than white Americans.{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|page=116}} The results were widely publicized by a lobby of anti-immigration activists, including the conservationist and theorist of ] ], who considered the so-called ] to be superior, but under threat because of immigration by "inferior breeds." In his influential work, ''A Study of American Intelligence,'' psychologist ] used the results of the Army tests to argue for a stricter immigration policy, limiting immigration to countries considered to belong to the "Nordic race".{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|pages=116, 309}} | |||
== Contemporary issues == | |||
{{Cleanup-section|date=May 2008}} | |||
In the 1920s, some US states enacted ] laws, such as Virginia's ], which established the ] (of ']') as law. Many scientists reacted negatively to eugenicist claims linking abilities and moral character to racial or genetic ancestry. They pointed to the contribution of environment (such as speaking English as a second language) to test results.{{sfn|Pickren|Rutherford|2010|p=163}} By the mid-1930s, many psychologists in the US had adopted the view that environmental and cultural factors played a dominant role in IQ test results. The psychologist Carl Brigham repudiated his own earlier arguments, explaining that he had come to realize that the tests were not a measure of innate intelligence.{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|page=145}} | |||
The most controversial and most publicized part of the debate is whether group IQ differences also reflect a genetic component. ] hypothesizes that a genetic contribution to intelligence could include genes linked to neuron structure or function, ] or metabolism, or other physiological differences which could vary with biogeographic ancestry. | |||
Discussions of the issue in the United States, especially in the writings of Madison Grant, influenced ] ] claims that the "Nordics" were a "]."{{sfn|Spiro|2009}} As American public sentiment shifted against the Germans, claims of racial differences in intelligence increasingly came to be regarded as problematic.<ref name="Ludy 2006">{{harvnb|Ludy|2006}}</ref> Anthropologists such as ], ], and ] did much to demonstrate that claims about racial hierarchies of intelligence were unscientific.{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|pages=130–32}} Nonetheless, a powerful eugenics and segregation lobby funded largely by textile-magnate ] continued to use intelligence studies as an argument for eugenics, segregation, and anti-immigration legislation.{{sfn|Tucker|2002}} | |||
=== The 1970s debates === | |||
Publication in 1969 of ] psychologist ]'s controversial article, "How Much Can We Boost IQ and School Achievement?" <ref>''Harvard Educational Review'' 39: 1-123</ref> triggered the modern debate. In it, he wrote, "All we are left with are various lines of evidence, no one of which is definitive alone, but which, viewed together, make it a not unreasonable hypothesis that genetic factors are strongly implicated in the average negro-white intelligence difference. The preponderance of evidence is, in my opinion, less consistent with a strictly environmental hypothesis than with a genetic hypothesis, which, of course, does not exclude the influence of environment or its interaction with genetic factors." ] ] wrote that Jensen's article was widely reported in the popular press "as an attempt to defend racism on scientific grounds".<ref>]'' 2nd edition by Peter Singer] 1999 Princeton University Press ISBN 978-0521439718</ref> | |||
=== The Pioneer Fund and ''The Bell Curve'' === | |||
An advocate of ], physicist ] focused on questions of race, intelligence, and ] and published several controversial papers arguing that intelligence is primarily hereditary.<ref></ref><ref></ref> He postulated that the higher reproduction rate of those with lower intelligence has a ] effect on society and proposed that individuals with ]s below 100 be paid to undergo voluntary ].<ref>{{cite book | last = Shockley | first = William | authorlink = William Shockley | title = Shockley on Eugenics and Race: The Application of Science to the Solution of Human Problems | isbn = 978-1878465030 | year = 1992 | publisher = Scott-Townsend Publishers | location = Washington, D.C.}}</ref> Biologists and geneticists criticized his theories, comparing them to rationale used by the ] in carrying out their genocidal policies. Criticism of Shockley's racial ideas appeared in scientific journals and was reflected in the popular press. | |||
As the desegregation of the American South gained traction in the 1950s, debate about black intelligence resurfaced. ], funded by Draper's ], published a new analysis of Yerkes' tests, concluding that black people really were of inferior intellect to white people. This study was used by segregationists to argue that it was to the advantage of black children to be educated separately from the superior white children.{{sfn|Jackson|2005}} In the 1960s, the debate was revived when ] publicly defended the view that black children were innately unable to learn as well as white children.{{sfn|Shurkin|2006}} ] expressed similar opinions in his '']'' article, "]," which questioned the value of ] for African-American children.{{sfn|Jensen|1969|pages=1–123}} He suggested that poor educational performance in such cases reflected an underlying genetic cause rather than lack of stimulation at home or other environmental factors.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Panofsky |first1=Aaron |title=Misbehaving Science. Controversy and the Development of Behavior Genetics |publisher=] |location=Chicago |isbn=978-0-226-05831-3 |date=2014}}</ref>{{sfn|Alland|2002|pages=79–80}} | |||
Another revival of public debate followed the appearance of '']'' (1994), a book by ] and ] that supported the general viewpoint of Jensen.{{sfn|Herrnstein|Murray|1994}} A statement in support of Herrnstein and Murray titled "]," was published in '']'' with 52 signatures. ''The Bell Curve'' also led to critical responses in a statement titled "]" of the American Psychological Association and in several books, including '']'' (1995), '']'' (1996) and a second edition of '']'' (1996) by ].<ref name="Maltby, Day & Macaskill 2007"/><ref name="Mackintosh 1998">{{harvnb|Mackintosh|1998}}</ref> | |||
Arguing that environmental factors could explain the black-white IQ gap,<ref>ISBN 0231133960.</ref> population geneticist ] debated Jensen and Shockley. | |||
Some of the authors proposing genetic explanations for group differences have received funding from the ], which was headed by ] until his death in 2012.{{sfn|Tucker|2002}}<ref name="Maltby, Day & Macaskill 2007">{{harvnb|Maltby|Day|Macaskill|2007}}</ref>{{sfn|Graves|2002a}}{{sfn|Graves|2002b}}<ref>{{harvnb|Grossman|Kaufman|2001}}</ref> Arthur Jensen, who jointly with Rushton published a 2005 review article arguing that the difference in average IQs between blacks and whites is partly due to genetics, received $1.1 million in grants from the Pioneer Fund.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Adam |first1=Miller |year=1994 |title=The Pioneer Fund: Bankrolling the Professors of Hate |url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/2962466 |journal=The Journal of Blacks in Higher Education |issue=6 |pages=58–61 |doi=10.2307/2962466|jstor=2962466 }}</ref><ref>{{cite web |last1=Blakemore |first1=Bill |last2=Jennings |first2=Peter |last3=Nissen |first3=Beth |date=November 22, 1994 |title=The Bell Curve and the Pioneer Fund |url=http://www.ferris.edu/isar/tanton/abcnews.htm |work=ABC World News Tonight |publisher=ABC News |access-date=May 1, 2020 |archive-date=March 3, 2016 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160303213542/http://www.ferris.edu/isar/tanton/abcnews.htm |url-status=live }} Vanderbilt Television News Archive : {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160103223437/http://tvnews.vanderbilt.edu/program.pl?ID=151406 |date=January 3, 2016 }}</ref> According to ], "The University of California's Arthur Jensen, cited twenty-three times in ''The Bell Curve''{{'}}s bibliography, is the book's principal authority on the intellectual inferiority of blacks."<ref>{{cite book |last=Montagu |first=Ashley |title=Race and IQ |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=New York |year=2002 |edition=2 |isbn=978-0-19-510221-5}}</ref> | |||
=== The 1990s debates === | |||
Discussion centers on whether group differences in average IQ are purely ], ], and ] or are hard-wired in ]. The ] has declared that "differentiating species into biologically defined 'races' has proven meaningless and unscientific as a way of explaining variation,"<ref> AAA statement on race and intelligence</ref> while the ] acknowledged inter-group IQ differences that can be influenced by ] factors.<ref name="Neisser, U. 1996">Neisser, U., G. Boodoo, T. J. Bouchard, A. W. Boykin, N. Brody, S. J. Ceci, D. F. Halpern, J. C. Loehlin, R. Perloff, R. J. Sternberg and S. Urbina. 1996. "Intelligence: knowns and unknowns." ''American Psychologist'' 51:77-101.</ref> | |||
The ] lists the Pioneer Fund as a ], citing the fund's history, its funding of race and intelligence research, and its connections with ] individuals.{{sfn|Berlet|2003}} Other researchers have criticized the Pioneer Fund for promoting ], ] and ].{{sfn|Tucker|2002}}<ref> {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110525150639/http://www.pioneerfund.org/Board.html |date=2011-05-25 }}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Falk|2008|p=18}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Wroe|2008|p=81}}</ref> | |||
Individuals such as ] and ] differ from the ] and ] in their claims that IQ score differences are traceable to genetics. ],<ref>{{citation|first=Stephen Jay|last=Gould|title=The Mismeasure of Man| publisher=Sagebrush Education Resources|year=1996|id=ISBN 0613181301}}</ref> ], ]<ref> {{citation| first=Richard|last=Lewontin|title=It Ain't Necessarily So: The Dream of the Human Genome and Other Illusions|publisher=New York review of Books|year=2001|id=ISBN 0940322951}}</ref> and ]<ref> by Joseph L Graves</ref> contend that the proponents of the genetics explanation are wrong. | |||
== Conceptual issues == | |||
The '']'' by ] ] and ] political scientist ] wrote: "The debate about whether and how much genes and environment have to do with ethnic differences remains unresolved. The universality of the contrast in nonverbal and verbal skills between East Asians and European whites suggests, without quite proving, genetic roots." Herrnstein and Murray said intelligence is a better predictor of many factors including financial income, job performance, unwed pregnancy, and crime than parents' ] or education level. | |||
=== Intelligence and IQ === | |||
''The Bell Curve'' attracted attention, both critical of and in defense of the book. Critics called it ]. Several books were written in response, including '']'' and '']'' (second edition) and scholarly associations released statements of opinion. | |||
{{Main|Human intelligence|Intelligence quotient|G factor (psychometrics)}} | |||
The concept of intelligence and the degree to which intelligence is measurable are matters of debate. There is no consensus about how to define intelligence; nor is it universally accepted that it is something that can be meaningfully measured by a single figure.<ref name="Schacter, Gilbert & Wegner 2007, pp 350-1">{{harvnb|Schacter|Gilbert|Wegner|2007|pp=350–1}}</ref> A recurring criticism is that different societies value and promote different kinds of skills and that the concept of intelligence is therefore culturally variable and cannot be measured by the same criteria in different societies.<ref name="Schacter, Gilbert & Wegner 2007, pp 350-1"/> Consequently, some critics argue that it makes no sense to propose relationships between intelligence and other variables.<ref name="Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd 2005">{{harvnb|Sternberg|Grigorenko|Kidd|2005}}</ref> | |||
Correlations between scores on various types of IQ tests led English psychologist ] to propose in 1904 the existence of an underlying factor, which he referred to as "''g''" or "]", a trait which is supposed to be innate.<ref name="deary2008">{{Cite journal |last1=Deary |first1=I. J. |last2=Lawn |first2=M. |last3=Bartholomew |first3=D. J. |year=2008 |title="A conversation between Charles Spearman, Godfrey Thomson, and Edward L. Thorndike: The International Examinations Inquiry Meetings 1931-1938": Correction to Deary, Lawn, and Bartholomew (2008) |url=https://www.research.ed.ac.uk/portal/files/8897614/a_conversation_between_charles_spareman.pdf |journal=History of Psychology |volume=11 |issue=3 |pages=156–157 |doi=10.1037/1093-4510.11.3.163 |hdl=20.500.11820/5417f3c7-e873-40b9-ad73-19c6acc9e35b |access-date=2020-06-25 |archive-date=2020-08-06 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200806163233/https://www.research.ed.ac.uk/portal/files/8897614/a_conversation_between_charles_spareman.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref> Another proponent of this view is ].{{sfn|Jensen|1998||page=}} This view, however, has been contradicted by a number of studies showing that education and changes in environment can significantly improve IQ test results.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Ceci |first=Stephen J. |date=1991 |title=How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence |journal=Developmental Psychology |volume=27 |issue=5 |pages=703–722 |doi=10.1037/0012-1649.27.5.703}}</ref>{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}}<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Richie |first1=Stuart J. |last2=Tucker-Drob |first2=Elliot |date=June 2018 |title=How Much Does Education Improve Intelligence? A Meta-Analysis |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/325832102 |journal=Psychological Science |volume=29 |issue=8}}</ref> | |||
The ]'s Board of Scientific Affairs in 1995 established a task force which produced a report, "]"<ref name="Neisser, U. 1996"/> The psychology association report authors wrote that IQ scores have high predictive validity for individual differences in school achievement, for adult occupational status, even when variables such as education and family background have been statistically controlled, and they said individual differences in intelligence are substantially influenced by genetics (75% in adults). Contrary to Herrnstein and Murray's findings, they wrote that prolonged malnutrition during childhood does have long-term intellectual effects. The APA report writers took issue with ''The Bell Curve'''s genetic implications: | |||
<blockquote>The differential between the mean intelligence test scores of Blacks and Whites (about one standard deviation, although it may be diminishing) does not result from any obvious biases in test construction and administration, nor does it simply reflect differences in socio-economic status. Explanations based on factors of caste and culture may be appropriate, but so far have little direct empirical support. There is certainly no such support for a genetic interpretation. At present, no one knows what causes this differential.<ref></ref></blockquote> The APA report concluded with a call for more reflection in debates on intelligence and for a "shared and sustained effort" for more research to answer the many unanswered questions that remain. | |||
Other psychometricians have argued that, whether or not there is such a thing as a general intelligence factor, performance on tests relies crucially on knowledge acquired through prior exposure to the types of tasks that such tests contain. This means that comparisons of test scores between persons with widely different life experiences and cognitive habits do not reveal their relative innate potentials.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|p=359}} | |||
Eleven critical responses appeared in the January 1997 issue of ''American Psychologist'', suggesting ways in which the APA report could have been improved. Only Lynn and Rushton "dispute the task force's conclusion that there is no direct evidence for a genetic interpretation of the Black—White IQ difference."<ref>Neisser, U. (1997). "Never a Dull Moment". ''American Psychologist'' 52: 79-81.</ref> | |||
=== Race === | |||
On December 13, 1994, psychologist ] and 51 specialists in intelligence and related fields asserted on the opinion page of the ] <ref name="gottfredson">Gottfredson, Linda (December 13, 1994), "Mainstream Science on Intelligence". '']'', p A18.</ref> that IQ bell curves differ across racial and ethnic groups for varying reasons. They maintained that, "Most experts believe that environment is important in pushing the bell curves apart, but that genetics could be involved too.<ref></ref> | |||
{{Main|Race (human categorization)|Race and genetics}} | |||
The consensus view among geneticists, biologists and anthropologists is that race is a sociopolitical phenomenon rather than a biological one,<ref name="NASEM-2023">{{Cite book |url=https://nap.nationalacademies.org/read/26902/chapter/1 |title=Using Population Descriptors in Genetics and Genomics Research: A New Framework for an Evolving Field (Consensus Study Report) |date=2023 |publisher=] |doi=10.17226/26902 |pmid=36989389 |isbn=978-0-309-70065-8 |quote=In humans, race is a socially constructed designation, a misleading and harmful surrogate for population genetic differences, and has a long history of being incorrectly identified as the major genetic reason for phenotypic differences between groups.}}</ref>{{sfn|Daley|Onwuegbuzie|2011|p=294}}<ref name="Templeton2016">Templeton, A. (2016). EVOLUTION AND NOTIONS OF HUMAN RACE. In Losos J. & Lenski R. (Eds.), ''How Evolution Shapes Our Lives: Essays on Biology and Society'' (pp. 346–361). Princeton; Oxford: Princeton University Press. {{doi|10.2307/j.ctv7h0s6j.26}}. That this view reflects the consensus among American anthropologists is stated in: {{cite journal |last1=Wagner |first1=Jennifer K. |last2=Yu |first2=Joon-Ho |last3=Ifekwunigwe |first3=Jayne O. |last4=Harrell |first4=Tanya M. |last5=Bamshad |first5=Michael J. |last6=Royal |first6=Charmaine D. |date=February 2017 |title=Anthropologists' views on race, ancestry, and genetics |journal=American Journal of Physical Anthropology |volume=162 |issue=2 |pages=318–327 |doi=10.1002/ajpa.23120 |pmc=5299519 |pmid=27874171}} See also: {{cite web |author=] |date=27 March 2019 |title=AAPA Statement on Race and Racism |url=https://physanth.org/about/position-statements/aapa-statement-race-and-racism-2019/ |access-date=19 June 2020 |website=American Association of Physical Anthropologists |archive-date=25 January 2022 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20220125163036/https://physanth.org/about/position-statements/aapa-statement-race-and-racism-2019/ |url-status=live }}</ref> a view supported by considerable genetics research.{{sfn|Smay|Armelagos|2000}}<ref>{{Cite journal |journal=Nature Genetics |date=2004 |volume=36 |issue=11 Suppl |pages=43–47 |author1=Rotimi, Charles N. |title=Are medical and nonmedical uses of large-scale genomic markers conflating genetics and 'race'? |doi=10.1038/ng1439 |quote="Two facts are relevant: (i) as a result of different evolutionary forces, including natural selection, there are geographical patterns of genetic variations that correspond, for the most part, to continental origin; and (ii) observed patterns of geographical differences in genetic information do not correspond to our notion of social identities, including 'race' and 'ethnicity" |pmid=15508002 |doi-access=free}}</ref> The current mainstream view is that race is a social construction based on folk ideologies that construct groups based on social disparities and superficial physical characteristics.<ref>{{harvnb|Schaefer|2008}}</ref> A 2023 consensus report from the ] stated: "In humans, race is a socially constructed designation, a misleading and harmful surrogate for population genetic differences, and has a long history of being incorrectly identified as the major genetic reason for phenotypic differences between groups."<ref name="NASEM-2023" /> | |||
The concept of human "races" as natural and separate divisions within the human species has also been rejected by the ]. The official position of the AAA, adopted in 1998, is that advances in scientific knowledge have made it "clear that human populations are not unambiguous, clearly demarcated, biologically distinct groups" and that "any attempt to establish lines of division among biological populations both arbitrary and subjective."<ref name="AAA">{{harvnb|AAA|1998}}</ref> A more recent statement from the ] (2019) declares that "Race does not provide an accurate representation of human biological variation. It was never accurate in the past, and it remains inaccurate when referencing contemporary human populations. Humans are not divided biologically into distinct continental types or racial genetic clusters."<ref>{{Cite web |title=AAPA Statement on Race & Racism |url=https://physanth.org/about/position-statements/aapa-statement-race-and-racism-2019/ |access-date=2020-06-28 |archive-date=2022-01-25 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20220125163036/https://physanth.org/about/position-statements/aapa-statement-race-and-racism-2019/ |url-status=live }}</ref> | |||
In 1994, the ] declared itself "deeply concerned by recent public discussions which imply that intelligence is biologically determined by race. Repeatedly challenged by scientists, nevertheless these ideas continue to be advanced. Such discussions distract public and scholarly attention from and diminish support for the collective challenge to ensure equal opportunities for all people, regardless of ethnicity or phenotypic variation."<ref></ref> | |||
Anthropologists such as ],<ref name="Brace 2005">{{harvnb|Brace|2005}}</ref> the philosophers Jonathan Kaplan and Rasmus Winther,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Kaplan |first1=Jonathan Michael |last2=Winther |first2=Rasmus Grønfeldt |date=2014 |title=Realism, Antirealism, and Conventionalism About Race |url=https://philpapers.org/rec/KAPRAA |journal=] |volume=81 |issue=5 |pages=1039–1052 |doi=10.1086/678314 |s2cid=55148854}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last=Winther |first=Rasmus Grønfeldt |date=2015 |title=The Genetic Reification of 'Race'?: A Story of Two Mathematical Methods |url=http://philpapers.org/archive/WINTGR.pdf |journal=] |volume=2 |issue=2 |pages=204–223}}</ref>{{sfnp|Kaplan|Winther|2013}} and the geneticist ],<ref>{{Cite web |last=Graves |first=Joseph |date=7 June 2006 |title=What We Know and What We Don't Know: Human Genetic Variation and the Social Construction of Race |url=http://raceandgenomics.ssrc.org/Graves/ |website=Race and Genomics |access-date=3 December 2023 |archive-date=3 June 2019 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190603030227/http://raceandgenomics.ssrc.org/Graves/ |url-status=dead }}</ref> have argued that the cluster structure of genetic data is dependent on the initial hypotheses of the researcher and the influence of these hypotheses on the choice of populations to sample. When one samples continental groups, the clusters become continental, but if one had chosen other sampling patterns, the clustering would be different. Weiss and Fullerton have noted that if one sampled only Icelanders, Mayans and Maoris, three distinct clusters would form and all other populations could be described as being clinally composed of admixtures of Maori, Icelandic and Mayan genetic materials.<ref name="evolutionary">{{cite journal |last1=Weiss |first1=K. M. |last2=Fullerton |first2=S. M. |date=2005 |title=Racing around, getting nowhere |journal=Evolutionary Anthropology |volume=14 |issue=5 |pages=165–169 |doi=10.1002/evan.20079 |s2cid=84927946}}</ref> Kaplan and Winther conclude that while racial groups are characterized by different allele frequencies, this does not mean that racial classification is a natural taxonomy of the human species, because multiple other genetic patterns can be found in human populations that crosscut racial distinctions. Moreover, the genomic data underdetermines whether one ]. Under Kaplan and Winther's view, racial groupings are objective social constructions (see Mills 1998<ref>{{cite book |last=Mills |first=C. W. |title=Blackness visible: essays on philosophy and race |date=1988 |publisher=] |location=Ithaca, New York |pages=41–66 |chapter=But What Are You Really? The Metaphysics of Race |author-link=C. Wright Mills}}</ref>) that have conventional biological reality only insofar as the categories are chosen and constructed for pragmatic scientific reasons. {{harvp|Sternberg|Grigorenko|Kidd|2005}} argue that the social construction of race derives not from any valid scientific basis but rather "from people's desire to classify."<ref name="Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd 2005" /> | |||
In August 1995, at the ] ] Sanders Korenman and ] sociologist ] found certain errors in Herrnstein's methodology. Korenman and Winship concluded: "... there is evidence of substantial bias due to measurement error in their estimates of the effects of parents` socioeconomic status. In addition, Herrnstein and Murray`s measure of parental socioeconomic status (SES) fails to capture the effects of important elements of family background (such as single-parent family structure at age 14). As a result, their analysis gives an exaggerated impression of the importance of IQ relative to parents` SES, and relative to family background more generally. Estimates based on a variety of methods, including analyses of siblings, suggest that parental family background is at least as important, and may be more important than IQ in determining socioeconomic success in adulthood."<ref> http://ssrn.com/abstract=225294 Korenman, Sanders and Winship, Christopher, "A Reanalysis of The Bell Curve" (August 1995). NBER Working Paper Series, Vol. w5230, 1995.</ref> | |||
In studies of human intelligence, race is almost always determined using self-reports rather than analyses of genetic characteristics. According to psychologist David Rowe, self-report is the preferred method for racial classification in studies of racial differences because classification based on genetic markers alone ignore the "cultural, behavioral, sociological, psychological, and epidemiological variables" that distinguish racial groups.<ref name="Rowe 2005">{{harvnb|Rowe|2005}}</ref> Hunt and Carlson disagreed, writing that "Nevertheless, self-identification is a surprisingly reliable guide to genetic composition," citing a study by {{harvp|Tang et al.|2005}}.<ref name="Hunt & Carlson 2007">{{harvnb|Hunt|Carlson|2007}}</ref> Sternberg and Grigorenko disputed Hunt and Carlson's interpretation of Tang's results as supporting the view that racial divisions are biological; rather, "Tang et al.'s point was that ancient geographic ancestry rather than current residence is associated with self-identification and not that such self-identification provides evidence for the existence of biological race."<ref>{{harvnb|Sternberg|Grigorenko|2007}}</ref> | |||
=== Policy debates === | |||
:''See also: ]'' | |||
Public policy implications of IQ and race research are one of the greatest sources of controversy surrounding this issue. Regardless of the source of the IQ gap, most educators agree that it must be addressed; they often advocate equitable funding for education.<ref></ref><ref></ref> An ] refers to the observed disparity on a number of ]al measures between the performance of groups of students, including groups defined by race or ]. | |||
== Group differences == | |||
Some proponents of a genetic interpretation of the IQ gap, such as Rushton and Jensen (2005a) and Gottfredson (2005b), have sometimes argued that their interpretation does not in itself demand any particular policy response: while a conservative/] commentator<ref> For example, the policy recommendations of '']'' were denounced by many.{{citation needed|date=February 2007}} Herrnstein and Murray (1994) wrote: "We can imagine no recommendation for using the government to manipulate fertility that does not have dangers. But this highlights the problem: The United States already has policies that inadvertently social-engineer who has babies, and it is encouraging the wrong women. ''If the United States did as much to encourage high-IQ women to have babies as it now does to encourage low-IQ women, it would rightly be described as engaging in aggressive manipulation of fertility.'' The technically precise description of America's fertility policy is that it subsidizes births among poor women, who are also disproportionately at the low end of the intelligence distribution. We urge generally that these policies, represented by the extensive network of cash and services for low-income women who have babies, be ended. (p. 548)" | |||
The study of human intelligence is one of the most controversial topics in psychology, in part because of difficulty reaching agreement about the meaning of ''intelligence'' and objections to the assumption that intelligence can be meaningfully measured by IQ tests. Claims that there are innate differences in intelligence between racial and ethnic groups—which go back at least to the 19th century—have been criticized for relying on specious assumptions and research methods and for serving as an ideological framework for discrimination and racism.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}}{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|p=222}} | |||
In a 2012 study of tests of different components of intelligence, Hampshire et al. expressed disagreement with the view of Jensen and Rushton that genetic factors must play a role in IQ differences between races, stating that "it remains unclear ... whether population differences in intelligence test scores are driven by heritable factors or by other correlated demographic variables such as socioeconomic status, education level, and motivation. More relevantly, it is questionable whether relate to a unitary intelligence factor, as opposed to a bias in testing paradigms toward particular components of a more complex intelligence construct."<ref name=":1">{{Harvnb|Hampshire|Highfield|Parkin|Owen|2012}}.</ref> According to Jackson and Weidman, | |||
Two year later the ] substantially cut these programs. In a discussion of the future political outcomes of an intellectually stratified society, they stated that they: "fear that a new kind of conservatism is becoming the dominant ideology of the affluent - not in the social tradition of an ] or in the economic tradition of an ] but ’conservatism’ along Latin American lines, where to be conservative has often meant doing whatever is necessary to preserve the mansions on the hills from the menace of the slums below. (p. 518)"Moreover, they fear that an increasing welfare will create a "custodial state": "a high-tech and more lavish version of the ] of some substantial minority of the nation’s population. They also predict increasing totalitarianism: It is difficult to imagine the United States preserving its heritage of individualism, equal rights before the law, free people running their own lives, once it is accepted that a significant part of the population must be made permanent wards of the states. (p. 526)"</ref> may feel the results justify, for example, reductions in ], a ] commentator may argue from a ] point of view (that genetic advantages are undeserved and unjust) for substantial affirmative action.<ref>Gottfredson 2005b</ref> Since all races have representatives at all levels of the IQ curve, this means any policy based on low IQ affects members of all races. | |||
{{blockquote|There are a number of reasons why the genetic argument for race differences in intelligence has not won many adherents in the scientific community. First, even taken on its own terms, the case made by Jensen and his followers did not hold up to scrutiny. Second, the rise of population genetics undercut the claims for a genetic cause of intelligence. Third, the new understanding of ] offered a better explanation for the existence of differences in IQ scores between the races.{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|p=222}}}} | |||
=== Test scores === | |||
In the book '']'', philosopher ] wrote that: <blockquote>Let us suppose that the genetic hypothesis turns out to be correct... I believe that the implications of this supposition are less drastic than they are often supposed to be... | |||
{{main|Achievement gap in the United States}} | |||
First, the genetic hypothesis does not imply that we should reduce our efforts to overcome other causes of inequality between people... Perhaps we should put special efforts into helping those who start from a position of disadvantage, so that we end with a more egalitarian result. | |||
In the United States, Asians on average score higher than White people, who tend to score higher than Hispanics, who tend to score higher than African Americans.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} Much greater variation in IQ scores exists within each ethnic group than between them.{{Clarify|reason=The prose here should be clarified. Is it saying that the recorded IQ range within each race is greater than any differences of averages between races?|date=December 2023}}<ref name="Reynolds-2021">{{Cite book |last1=Reynolds |first1=Cecil R. |chapter-url=https://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-3-030-59455-8_15 |title=Mastering Modern Psychological Testing |last2=Altmann |first2=Robert A. |last3=Allen |first3=Daniel N. |publisher=Springer |year=2021 |pages=573–613, 582 |chapter=The Problem of Bias in Psychological Assessment|doi=10.1007/978-3-030-59455-8_15 |isbn=978-3-030-59454-1 |s2cid=236660997 }}</ref><ref name=SAGE>{{Cite book |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=yDDqLBBk7BcC |title=Encyclopedia of Diversity in Education |date=2012 |publisher=SAGE |isbn=978-1-4129-8152-1 |page=1209 |language=en |access-date=2018-01-20 |archive-date=2023-03-20 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230320043631/https://books.google.com/books?id=yDDqLBBk7BcC |url-status=live }}</ref> A 2001 ] of the results of 6,246,729 participants tested for cognitive ability or aptitude found a difference in average scores between black people and white people of 1.1 ]. Consistent results were found for college and university application tests such as the ] (N = 2.4 million) and ] (N = 2.3 million), as well as for tests of job applicants in corporate settings (N = 0.5 million) and in the military (N = 0.4 million).<ref name="Roth et al. 2001">{{harvnb|Roth et al.|2001}}</ref> | |||
Second, the fact that the average IQ of one racial group is a few points higher than that of another does not allow anyone to say that all members of the higher IQ group have higher IQs... The point is that these figures are averages and say nothing about individuals... | |||
The third reason... is simply that, as we saw earlier, the principle of equality is not based on any actual equality that all people share. I have argued that the only defensible basis for the principle of equality is equal consideration of interests... Equal status does not depend on intelligence. Racists who maintain the contrary are in peril of being forced to kneel before the next genius they encounter. | |||
</blockquote> | |||
In response to the controversial 1994 book '']'', the ] (APA) formed a task-force of eleven experts, which issued a report "]" in 1996.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} Regarding group differences, the report reaffirmed the consensus that differences within groups are much wider than differences between groups, and that claims of ethnic differences in intelligence should be scrutinized carefully, as such claims had been used to justify racial discrimination. The report also acknowledged problems with the racial categories used, as these categories are neither consistently applied, nor homogeneous {{xref|(see ])}}.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} | |||
Policies focused on geographical regions or nations may have disproportionate influences on certain racial groups and on cognitive development.{{Who|date=November 2008}} Differences in health care, nutrition, regulation of environmental toxins, and geographic distribution of diseases and control strategies between the developing world and developed nations have all been subjects of policies or policy recommendations (see ]). | |||
In the UK, some African groups have higher average educational attainment and standardized test scores than the overall population.<ref>{{cite journal |first1=Feyisa |last1=Demie |first2=Christabel |last2=McLean |title=Raising the achievement of African heritage pupils: a case study of good practice in British schools |journal=Educational Studies |date=1 December 2007 |issn=0305-5698 |pages=415–434 |volume=33 |issue=4 |doi=10.1080/03055690701423606 |s2cid=144579288}}</ref> In 2010–2011, white British pupils were 2.3% less likely to have gained 5 A*–C grades at ] than the national average, whereas the likelihood was 21.8% above average for those of ] origin, 5.5% above average for those of ] origin, and 1.4% above average for those of ] origin. For the two other African ethnic groups on which data was available, the likelihood was 23.7% below average for those of ] origin and 35.3% below average for those of ] origin.<ref>{{Cite book |last=Rutter |first=Jill |title=Back to basics: Towards a successful and cost-effective integration policy |work=IPPR |publisher=Institute for Public Policy Research |year=2013 |url=https://www.ippr.org/publications/back-to-basics-towards-a-successful-and-cost-effective-integration-policy |page=43 |access-date=2020-05-23 |archive-date=2020-04-13 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200413214928/https://www.ippr.org/publications/back-to-basics-towards-a-successful-and-cost-effective-integration-policy |url-status=live }}</ref> In 2014, Black-African pupils of 11 language groups were more likely to pass ] Maths 4+ in England than the national average. Overall, the average pass rate by ethnicity was 86.5% for white British (N = 395,787), whereas it was 85.6% for Black-Africans (N = 18,497). Nevertheless, several Black-African language groups, including ], ], ], ], ], ], ], ], ] speakers, and English-speaking Africans, each had an average pass rate above the white British average (total N = 9,314), with the Hausa, Igbo, Yoruba, and Amhara having averages above 90% (N = 2,071).<ref name=":0">{{Cite book |last=Feyisa Demie |first=Andrew Hau |title=Language Diversity and Attainment in Primary Schools in England |publisher=Lambeth Research And Statistics Unit |year=2016 |url=https://www.lambeth.gov.uk/rsu/sites/www.lambeth.gov.uk.rsu/files/language_diversity_and_attainment_in_primary_schools_in_england_2017.pdf |page=18 |access-date=2020-05-24 |archive-date=2020-08-06 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200806165229/https://www.lambeth.gov.uk/rsu/sites/www.lambeth.gov.uk.rsu/files/language_diversity_and_attainment_in_primary_schools_in_england_2017.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref> In 2017–2018, the percentage of pupils getting a strong pass (grade 5 or above) in the English and maths GCSE (in ]) was 42.7% for whites (N = 396,680) and 44.3% for Black-Africans (N = 18,358).<ref>{{Cite web |title=GCSE English and maths results |url=https://www.ethnicity-facts-figures.service.gov.uk/education-skills-and-training/11-to-16-years-old/a-to-c-in-english-and-maths-gcse-attainment-for-children-aged-14-to-16-key-stage-4/3.0 |date=2019 |website=Gov.UK |access-date=2022-09-20 |archive-date=2022-09-20 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20220920173733/https://www.ethnicity-facts-figures.service.gov.uk/education-skills-and-training/11-to-16-years-old/a-to-c-in-english-and-maths-gcse-attainment-for-children-aged-14-to-16-key-stage-4/3.0 |url-status=live }}</ref> | |||
== Test score difference == | |||
=== United States === | |||
The observed differences in average test score achievement between ethnic groups varies depending on the populations studied and the type of tests used. Self defined black and white United States citizens have been the subjects of the greatest number of studies. Black-White average IQ differences appear to increase with age, averaging nearly 17 points by age 24.<ref name = "IQ-gap"></ref> According to James Flynn, the overall average Black-White gap has reduced by one third over the last 30 years.<ref name = "IQ-gap"/> A large (21,260 children) and probably the most recent (1998) study found that the Black-White gap for young children in reading and math scores was much smaller than in earlier studies.<ref>Roland G. Fryer Jr. and Steven D. Levitt, "," ''The Review of Economics and Statistics'' 86, no. 2 (2004).</ref> Black-White differences on ] shrunk over the last 30 years, but some of the improvements did not continue through the 1990s. The average black-white IQ difference also varies depending on test content. For example, two subsections of the ] IQ test, known as forward and reverse digit-span, ask children to repeat a long series of numbers either forwards or backwards. The black-white difference on forward digit span is relatively small, while the difference on reverse digit span is relatively large. Across a battery of tests, the size of the Black-White gap is correlated with the extent to which the tests measure the psychometric factor ''g'', which also accounts for most of the variation in interindividual differences in IQ test performance.<ref>{{cite book | author = Arthur Jensen | title = The g Factor | year = 1998 }}</ref> Using a variety of statistical techniques, Dolan and colleagues have found that the black-white IQ gap can be accounted for by differences in ''g'' and the other interindividual ability factors measured by IQ tests, and also that IQ tests measure roughly the same mix of abilities in both black and white populations.<ref>Dolan, C. V. (1997). A note on Schönemann's refutation of Spearman's hypothesis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32, 319-325.</ref><ref>Dolan, C. V. (2000). Investigating Spearman's hypothesis by means of multi-group confirmatory factor analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 35, 21-50.</ref><ref>Dolan, C. V., & Hamaker, E. L. (2001). Investigating black-white differences in psychometric IQ: Multi-group confirmatory factor analyses of WISC-R and K-ABC and a critique of the method of correlated vectors. In F. Columbus (Ed.), Advances in psychology research (Vol. 6, pp. 30-59). Huntington, NY: Nova Science.</ref> Gaps are seen in other tests of cognitive ability or aptitude, including university admission exams such as the ] and ] as well as employment tests for corporate settings and the military.<ref>Roth et al. 2001</ref> | |||
=== Flynn effect and the closing gap === | |||
The IQ distributions of other racial and ethnic groups in the United States are less well studied. Hispanic and Native American populations, including Arctic Natives,<ref>Berry, J. W. (1966). Temne and Eskimo perceptual skills. International Journal of Psychology, 1, 207-222.</ref><ref>MacArthur, R. S. (1968). Some differential abilities of northern Canadian native youth. International Journal of Psychology, 3, 43-51.</ref> tend to score worse on average than White populations but better on average than Black populations.<ref>Roth, P. L., Bevier, C. A., Bobko, P., Switzer, F. S. III, & Tyler, P. (2001). Ethnic group differences in cognitive ability in employment and educational settings: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 54, 297-330.</ref> East Asian populations may score higher on average than White populations in the United States as they do elsewhere.<ref name="autogenerated2">Hunt, Earl & Carlson, Jerry. Considerations Relating to the Study of Group Differences in Intelligence. Perspectives on Psychological Science 2 (2), 194-213.</ref> A 1960 study of 1236 American teenagers calculated six IQ measures for Jews relative to white gentiles. The results found that the relative IQ of American Jews varied from a low of 91.3 (visual reasoning) to a high of 109.7 (Mathematics).<ref>Backman, M.E. (1972) Patterns of mental abilities: ethnic, socioeconomic and sex differences. American Educational Research Journal, 9,1-12.</ref><ref name="Lynn, Richard 2003">Lynn, Richard (2003). The Intelligence of American Jews. Elsevier Ltd. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00079-5.</ref> A recent review by Lynn (2004) used a 10 word vocabulary test to estimate the IQ of American Jews. The population of 150 Jews scored half a standard deviation above the 5300 white gentiles in verbal IQ.<ref name="Lynn, Richard 2003"/><ref>Cochran, Gregory & Hardy, Jason & Harpending, Henry. Natural History of Ashkenazi Intelligence. Journal of Biosocial Science, Vol. 38, No. 05. (2005), pp. 659-693.</ref> | |||
{{Main|Flynn effect}} | |||
The ']' — a term coined after researcher ] — refers to the substantial rise in raw IQ test scores observed in many parts of the world during the 20th century. In the United States, the increase was continuous and approximately linear from the earliest years of testing to about 1998 when the gains stopped and some tests even showed decreasing test scores. For example, the average scores of black people on some IQ tests in 1995 were the same as the scores of white people in 1945.<ref>{{harvnb|Mackintosh|1998|p=162}}</ref> As one pair of academics phrased it, "the typical African American today probably has a slightly higher IQ than the grandparents of today's average white American."<ref>{{cite book |last=Swain |first=Carol |title=Contemporary voices of white nationalism in America |publisher=Cambridge University Press |location=Cambridge, UK New York |year=2003 |isbn=978-0-521-01693-3 |page= |url=https://archive.org/details/contemporaryvoic00swai/page/70}} Note: this quote is from the authors' introductory essay, not from the interviews.</ref> | |||
Flynn himself argued that the dramatic changes having taken place between one just generation and the next pointed strongly at an environmental explanation, and that it is highly unlikely that genetic factors could have accounted for the increasing scores. The Flynn effect, along with Flynn's analysis, continues to hold significance in the context of the black/white IQ gap debate, demonstrating the potential for environmental factors to influence IQ test scores by as much as 1 standard deviation, a scale of change that had previously been doubted.{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2001}} | |||
For each of these populations, there is some evidence that the mixture of ability factors that distinguish individuals are differentially distributed between groups. For example, East Asian populations tend to outscore White populations in performance IQ, whereas the test score differences skew towards higher verbal IQ for Ashkenazi-White differences. However, the mixture of abilities within groups appears to be nearly identical across many ethnic groups.<ref>Carretta, T. R., & Ree, M. J. (1995). Near identity of cognitive structure in sex and ethnic groups. Personality and Individual Differences, 19, 149-155.</ref> The stability of these differences is also less well studied than Black-White differences. | |||
A distinct but related observation has been the gradual narrowing of the American black-white IQ gap in the last decades of the 20th century, as black test-takers increased their average scores relative to white test-takers. For instance, Vincent reported in 1991 that the black–white IQ gap was decreasing among children, but that it was remaining constant among adults.{{sfn|Vincent|1991}} Similarly, a 2006 study by Dickens and Flynn estimated that the difference between mean scores of black people and white people closed by about 5 or 6 IQ points between 1972 and 2002,{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}} a reduction of about one-third. In the same period, the educational achievement disparity also diminished.<ref>Neisser, Ulric (Ed). 1998. The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association</ref> Reviews by Flynn and Dickens,{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}} Mackintosh,{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011}} and Nisbett ''et al.'' accept the gradual closing of the gap as a fact.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}} Flynn and Dickens summarize this trend, stating, "The constancy of the Black-White IQ gap is a myth and therefore cannot be cited as evidence that the racial IQ gap is genetic in origin."{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}} | |||
=== Worldwide === | |||
According to ], ], and others, IQ test score differences are observed cross-culturally and around the world. Lynn has published three books summarizing IQ test scores from around the world.<ref>], ], and ]</ref> The inaccuracy of the cross cultural IQ scores is well documented, but many scholars use the results as an estimate of worldwide IQ scores.<ref></ref><ref></ref><ref></ref><ref name = "doi.org-intell"></ref> Lynn's meta-analysis lists ]ns (105), ] (99), ] (91), ]ns and ] (87 each), ] (85), ]erners (including ]ns and ]ns) (84), ]ns (67), and ] (62).<ref>Herrnstein and Murray 1994; Lynn 1991a; Lynn 2006</ref><ref name="Rushton-review">{{cite journal | author = Rushton, J. P. | year = 2006 | month = | title = Lynn Richard, Race Differences in Intelligence: An Evolutionary Analysis, Washington Summit Books, Augusta, GA (2005) ISBN 1-59368-020-1, 318 pages., US$34.95 | journal = Personality and Individual Differences | volume = 40 | issue = 4 | pages = 853–855 | doi = 10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.004 | url = }}</ref><ref name="main">Lynn, R. and Vanhanen, T. (2002). IQ and the wealth of nations. Westport, CT: Praeger. ISBN 0-275-97510-X</ref> International achievement test scores, including ] and ], have also been used to estimate average IQ worldwide with similar results where data is available.<ref name = "doi.org-intell"/><ref>Rindermann, H. (2006). What do international student assessments measure?. Psychologische Rundschau, 57, 69–86.</ref><ref></ref><ref></ref> The very low IQ scores reported for sub-Saharan African populations is especially controversial. For example, Wicherts argues that the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans is poorly measured and is more likely 78.<ref>Wicherts, J.M. (December 15, 2006). The dark past, obscure present, and bright future of African IQ. 7th Annual Conference of the International Society for Intelligence Research (ISIR), San Francisco, CA, US.</ref> Sternberg, a critic of Rushton and Lynn, reported an average IQ of 70 for "young adolescents" in Kenya.<ref>Sternberg, R. J., Nokes, C., Geissler, P. W., Prince, R., Okatcha, F., Bundy, D. A., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2001). The relationship between academic and practical intelligence: A case study in Kenya. Intelligence, 29, 401-418.</ref> There is a paradox from IQ studies in Africa that has yet to be resolved. Europeans with an IQ of 70 are considered ]. However, the Africans found to have an IQ of 70 were perfectly normal with the apparent ability to function normally in their social environment. This has led to questions of the validity of these IQ tests<ref>{{cite book|chapterurl=http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=llFQEF2bkWAC&oi=fnd&pg=PR9&dq=iq+70+africa+mentally+retarded+miele&ots=Ymfc4tDAZ9&sig=LvLhMdTqV4QR4UquNt9bAx6BlkM#PPA225,M1| | |||
chapter=Race and Behavior|title=Race|last=Sarich|year=2004|isbn=0813340861|publisher=Westview Press|location=Boulder, Colo.}}</ref><ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.epjournal.net/filestore/ep03255262.pdf|format=PDF|title= A review of Race: the Reality of Human Differences|last=Nathan|year=2004]}}</ref> | |||
A 1995 study published in the Journal of Nutrition concluded that IQ scores of children in the West Indies are depressed to some extent by environmental conditions, such as macronutrient and micronutrient deficiencies<ref></ref><ref></ref> and even blood lead levels.<ref></ref> The study suggested the existence of broader links between malnutrition and IQ levels across the developing world as a whole.<ref></ref> | |||
==Environmental factors== | |||
Southern Italian immigrants in the US scored below 80 on IQ tests in the early 20th century<ref></ref> but had improved their average scores in later decades, bringing the validity of theories of genetically fixed and racially based IQ into further question. | |||
===Health and nutrition=== | |||
{{Main|Impact of health on intelligence}} | |||
] | |||
Environmental factors including ],<ref name="Bellinger, Stiles & Needleman 1992"/> low rates of ],<ref name="Campbell et al. 2002">{{harvnb|Campbell et al.|2002}}</ref> and poor ]<ref>{{harvnb|Ivanovic et al.|2004}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Saloojee| Pettifor|2001}}</ref> are significantly correlated with poor cognitive development and functioning. For example, childhood exposure to {{nowrap|lead{{tsp}}{{mdash}}{{tsp}}}}associated with homes in poorer {{nowrap|areas<ref>{{Cite web |url=https://www.atsdr.cdc.gov/csem/ped_env_health/docs/ped_env_health.pdf |title=Principles of Pediatric Environmental Health, The Child as Susceptible Host: A Developmental Approach to Pediatric Environmental Medicine |last=Agency For Toxic Substances And Disease Registry Case Studies In Environmental Medicine (CSEM) |date=2012-02-15 |website=U.S. Department for Health and Human Services |access-date=2019-01-30 |archive-date=2019-01-31 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190131093309/https://www.atsdr.cdc.gov/csem/ped_env_health/docs/ped_env_health.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref>{{hsp}}{{mdash}}{{tsp}}}}correlates with an average IQ drop of 7 points,<ref name="Lanphear Hornung Khoury Yolton 2005 pp. 894–899">{{cite journal |last1=Lanphear |first1=Bruce P. |last2=Hornung |first2=Richard |last3=Khoury |first3=Jane |last4=Yolton |first4=Kimberly |last5=Baghurst |first5=Peter |last6=Bellinger |first6=David C. |last7=Canfield |first7=Richard L. |last8=Dietrich |first8=Kim N. |last9=Bornschein |first9=Robert |last10=Greene |first10=Tom |last11=Rothenberg |first11=Stephen J. |last12=Needleman |first12=Herbert L. |last13=Schnaas |first13=Lourdes |last14=Wasserman |first14=Gail |last15=Graziano |first15=Joseph |last16=Roberts |first16=Russell |title=Low-Level Environmental Lead Exposure and Children's Intellectual Function: An International Pooled Analysis |journal=Environmental Health Perspectives |volume=113 |issue=7 |date=2005-03-18 |issn=0091-6765 |pmid=16002379 |pmc=1257652 |doi=10.1289/ehp.7688 |pages=894–899|bibcode=2005EnvHP.113..894L }}</ref> and ], on average, of 12 IQ points.<ref>{{harvnb|Qian et al.|2005}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |first1=James |last1=Feyrer |first2=Dimitra |last2=Politi |first3=David N. |last3=Weil |title=The Cognitive Effects of Micronutrient Deficiency: Evidence from Salt Iodization in the United States |year=2017 |journal=Journal of the European Economic Association |volume=15 |issue=2 |pages=355–387 |doi=10.1093/jeea/jvw002 |pmid=31853231 |pmc=6919660 |url=http://www.nber.org/papers/w19233.pdf |access-date=2019-07-22 |archive-date=2020-08-13 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200813174601/https://www.nber.org/papers/w19233.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref> Such impairments may sometimes be permanent, but in some cases they be partially or wholly compensated for by later growth. | |||
== Genetic and environmental factors == | |||
The cause(s) of group average IQ test score differences are not known but ] have been proposed. Many scholars have offered descriptions of the variety of hypotheses that have been proposed. These descriptions usually distinguish between those hypotheses which invoke a contribution of genetic factors and those which solely invoke environmental (i.e., non-genetic) factors. Some descriptions of the positions are themselves controversial. In a review published in 2007, Hunt and Carlson listed four positions.<ref name="autogenerated1"></ref> The first position, attributed to Jensen and Rushton, is that group differences in IQ reflect differences in intelligence that are "due in substantial part to genetically determined differences in brain structure and/or function"<ref name="autogenerated1" /> The second position, attributed to Ogbu and Sowell, is that the differences in intelligence test scores are due to social factors. The third view, attributed to Sternberg and colleagues, is that the use of IQ scores to argue for differences in intelligence is an inappropriate use of tests in different groups. The fourth position, attributed to Fish and others, is that there is no such thing as race: "a term motivated by social concerns and not a scientific concept".<ref name="autogenerated1" /> | |||
The first two years of life are critical for malnutrition, the consequences of which are often irreversible and include poor cognitive development, educability, and future economic productivity.<ref> {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110717005704/http://www.thelancet.com/series/maternal-and-child-undernutrition |date=2011-07-17 }}, 2008.</ref><!--Which paper? Link points only to to listed series of papers (yes, need more specific, preferably secondary, reference) --> Mackintosh points out that, for American black people, infant mortality is about twice as high as for white people, and low birth weight is twice as prevalent. At the same time, white mothers are twice as likely to breastfeed their infants, and breastfeeding is directly correlated with IQ for low-birth-weight infants. In this way, a wide number of health-related factors which influence IQ are unequally distributed between the two groups.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|pages=343–44}} | |||
The ] in 2004 stated that lack of both iodine and iron has been implicated in impaired brain development, and this can affect enormous numbers of people: it is estimated that one-third of the total global population is affected by ]. In developing countries, it is estimated that 40% of children aged four and under have ] because of insufficient iron in their diets.<ref>{{harvnb|Behrman|Alderman|Hoddinott|2004}}</ref> | |||
Prominent proponents of the genetic hypothesis are predominately psychologists or experts in intelligence testing, such as ], ], ], ], ], ], and ]. A few prominent proponents have come from other fields, such as political scientist ], evolutionary psychologist ], and philosophers ] and ]. Not all proponents share the same views, nor have all of their views been constant throughout their careers. Prominent critics come from many backgrounds, and hold a diversity of views. They include psychologists and experts in intelligence testing such as ] (prior to his work on IQ Flynn was a political scientist), ], ], and a number of less well known ] who criticize the underlying IQ test data. Other critics include economists ] and ]. ] such as ], ], and ] argue that "race" is a social and not a biological concept. For example Marks (1996) argues that the eugenics movement of the 1920s identified Ashkenazi Jews from Europe as intellectually inferior due to their genetics{{Request quotation|date=June 2009}}{{Page needed}}, but that Murray and Herrnstein claim this group as a "cognitive elite" in ''The Bell Curve'', concluding that "race" is a social construct that does not reflect underlying biology.<ref>Marks, J. (1996) "Science and Race", ''American Behavioral Scientist'' '''40:''' 123-133. {{doi|10.1177/0002764296040002003}}</ref> Sociologists ], ] and ], paleontologist ], geneticist ] and evolutionary biologist and physiologist ] also oppose the genetic hypothesis. | |||
Other scholars have found that simply the standard of nutrition has a significant effect on population intelligence, and that the Flynn effect may be caused by increasing nutrition standards across the world.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Colom |first1=R. |last2=Lluis-Font |first2=J. M. |last3=Andrés-Pueyo |first3=A. |year=2005 |title=The generational intelligence gains are caused by decreasing variance in the lower half of the distribution: supporting evidence for the nutrition hypothesis |journal=Intelligence |volume=33 |pages=83–91 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.010}}</ref> James Flynn has himself argued against this view.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Flynn |first1=J. R. |year=2009a |title=Requiem for nutrition as the cause of IQ gains: Raven's gains in Britain 1938 to 2008 |journal=Economics and Human Biology |volume=7 |issue=1 |pages=18–27 |doi=10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.009 |pmid=19251490}}</ref> | |||
The genetic hypothesis has been characterized as inherently racist. Eric Turkheimer, a professor of clinical psychology at the University of Virginia, argues that proponents of a genetic contribution, whom he calls "racialists", deserve "vigorous disapprobation" and do not deserve "respect".<ref name="autogenerated5"></ref> Turkheimer writes that he disagrees with his colleagues, such as Flynn and Ceci, who "agree that the question is a legitimate matter for scientific inquiry, to be settled by cool-headed evaluation of the empirical evidence".<ref name="autogenerated5" /> Flynn does not accept Turkheimer's arguments.<ref></ref> Rushton and Jensen argue that the policy issues should be regarded as separate from the academic inquiry.<ref></ref> | |||
Some recent research has argued that the retardation caused in brain development by ]s, many of which are more prevalent in non-white populations, may be an important factor in explaining the differences in IQ between different regions of the world. The findings of this research, showing the correlation between IQ, race and infectious diseases was also shown to apply to the IQ gap in the US, suggesting that this may be an important environmental factor.<ref name="Eppig 2011">{{harvnb|Eppig|2011}}</ref> | |||
=== What is heritability? === | |||
{{See also|Heritability of IQ}} | |||
]. The height of this "ordinary genetically varied corn" is 100% heritable, but the difference between the groups is totally environmental. This is because the nutrient solution varies between populations, but not within populations.<ref></ref>]] | |||
A 2013 meta-analysis by the World Health Organization found that, after controlling for maternal IQ, breastfeeding was associated with IQ gains of 2.19 points. The authors suggest that this relationship is causal but state that the practical significance of this gain is debatable; however, they highlight one study suggesting an association between breastfeeding and academic performance in Brazil, where "breastfeeding duration does not present marked variability by socioeconomic position."<ref>{{Cite web |title=Long-term effects of breastfeeding – a systemic review |first1=Bernardo L. |last1=Horta |first2=Cesar G. |last2=Victoria |publisher=World Health Organization |year=2013 |access-date=18 June 2018 |url=http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/79198/9789241505307_eng.pdf |archive-date=9 April 2020 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200409233115/https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/79198/9789241505307_eng.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref> Colen and Ramey (2014) similarly find that controlling for sibling comparisons within families, rather than between families, reduces the correlation between breastfeeding status and WISC IQ scores by nearly a third, but further find the relationship between breastfeeding duration and WISC IQ scores to be insignificant. They suggest that "much of the beneficial long-term effects typically attributed to breastfeeding, per se, may primarily be due to selection pressures into infant feeding practices along key demographic characteristics such as race and socioeconomic status."<ref>{{cite journal |first1=Cynthia G. |last1=Colen |first2=David M. |last2=Ramey |journal=Social Science & Medicine |volume=109 |issue=1 |pages=55–65 |year=2014 |pmc=4077166 |title=Is Breast Truly Best? Estimating the Effect of Breastfeeding on Long-term Child Wellbeing in the United States Using Sibling Comparisons |doi=10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.01.027 |pmid=24698713}}</ref> Reichman estimates that no more than 3 to 4% of the black–white IQ gap can be explained by black–white disparities in low birth weight.<ref>{{harvnb|Reichman|2005}}</ref> | |||
The consensus among intelligence researchers is that IQ differences between individuals of the same racial-ethnic group reflect real, functionally and socially significant, and substantially heritable differences in intelligence.<ref>R. J. Sternberg (2000) Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press</ref><ref>David J. Bartholomew (2004) Measuring Intelligence: Facts and Fallacies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press</ref><ref>Ian J. Deary. (2001) Intelligence: A Very Short Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press</ref><ref>Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., Halpern, D. F., Loehlin, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J. and Urbina, S. (February 1996). "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns". American Psychologist 51 (2): 77-101.</ref><ref>Gottfredson, L. S. (Ed.). (1997). Intelligence and social policy . Intelligence, 24(1).</ref><ref> Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Mainstream science on intelligence: An editorial with 52 signatories, history, and bibliography. Intelligence, 24(1), 13-23.</ref><ref>Robert Plomin, John C. DeFries, Gerald E. McClearn, and Peter McGuffin (2000) Behavioral Genetics. Worth Publishers; Fourth Edition edition</ref><ref>Brody, N. (1992). Intelligence (2nd ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.</ref><ref>Snyderman, M., & Rothman, S. (1988). The IQ controversy, the media and public policy. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Press.</ref> | |||
===Education=== | |||
] for IQ from twin studies and other study designs consistently fall in the range of 50% to 80%, with the estimated ] in young (preschool) children in the lower range and adults in the higher range. | |||
Several studies have proposed that a large part of the gap in IQ test performance can be attributed to differences in quality of education.<ref>{{harvnb|Manly et al.|2002}} and {{harvnb|Manly et al.|2004}}</ref> ] in education has been proposed as one possible cause of differences in educational quality between races.<ref>{{harvnb|Mickelson|2003}}</ref> According to a paper by Hala Elhoweris, Kagendo Mutua, Negmeldin Alsheikh and Pauline Holloway, teachers' referral decisions for students to participate in ] educational programs were influenced in part by the students' ethnicity.<ref>{{harvnb|Elhoweris et al.|2005}}</ref> | |||
The ], an intensive early childhood education project, was also able to bring about an average IQ gain of 4.4 points at age 21 in the black children who participated in it compared to controls.<ref name="Campbell et al. 2002"/> ] agreed that the Abecedarian project demonstrated that education can have a significant effect on IQ, but also declared his view that no educational program thus far had been able to reduce the black–white IQ gap by more than a third, and that differences in education are thus unlikely to be its only cause.<ref>{{harvnb|Miele|2002|p=133}}</ref> | |||
Critics have also questioned the interpretation of heritability as a whole. Lewontin suggests that some genotypes are more influenced by environments than others, leading to the possibility that populations that have similar genetic variance in the same environment can have different heritabilities because of their different genotypes.<ref name="lewontin70">Lewontin, R. (1970) "Race and Intelligence". ''Science and Public Affairs'' March, pp. 2-8</ref> David Layzer (1974) contends that the development of a trait can be influenced by genetic differences ]ly and that heritability estimates cannot measure such qualitative differences, as such it is possible that even with a heritability of close to 100% it is possibly for ] variance to be due largely to environment.<ref name="layzer">Layzer, David. (1974) "Heritability analyses of IQ scores: Science or numerology?" ''Science'' '''183''' pp. 1259-66</ref> | |||
A series of studies by ] and Cynthia Holland measured the effect of prior exposure to the kind of cognitive tasks posed in IQ tests on test performance. Assuming that the IQ gap was the result of lower exposure to tasks using the cognitive functions usually found in IQ tests among African American test takers, they prepared a group of African Americans in this type of tasks before taking an IQ test. The researchers found that there was no subsequent difference in performance between the African-Americans and white test takers.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Fagan |first1=Joseph F |last2=Holland |first2=Cynthia R |year=2002 |title=Equal opportunity and racial differences in IQ |journal=Intelligence |volume=30 |issue=4 |pages=361–387 |doi=10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00080-6}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Fagan |first1=J.F. |last2=Holland |first2=C.R. |year=2007 |title=Racial equality in intelligence: Predictions from a theory of intelligence as processing |journal=Intelligence |volume=35 |issue=4 |pages=319–334 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.08.009}}</ref> Daley and Onwuegbuzie conclude that Fagan and Holland demonstrate that "differences in knowledge between black people and white people for intelligence test items can be erased when equal opportunity is provided for exposure to the information to be tested".{{sfn|Daley|Onwuegbuzie|2011}} A similar argument is made by ] who argues that IQ differences correlate well with differences in literacy suggesting that developing literacy skills through education causes an increase in IQ test performance.<ref name="Marks, D.F. 2010">{{cite journal |last1=Marks |first1=D.F. |year=2010 |title=IQ variations across time, race, and nationality: An artifact of differences in literacy skills |journal=Psychological Reports |volume=106 |issue=3 |pages=643–664 |doi=10.2466/pr0.106.3.643-664 |pmid=20712152 |s2cid=12179547}}</ref><ref name="psychologytoday.com">{{cite magazine |last=Barry |first=Scott |url=http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/beautiful-minds/201008/the-flynn-effect-and-iq-disparities-among-races-ethnicities-and-nations- |title=The Flynn Effect and IQ Disparities Among Races, Ethnicities, and Nations: Are There Common Links? |magazine=Psychology Today |date=2010-08-23 |access-date=2014-08-22 |archive-date=2023-03-20 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230320043730/https://www.psychologytoday.com/intl/blog/beautiful-minds/201008/the-flynn-effect-and-iq-disparities-among-races-ethnicities-and-nations |url-status=live }}</ref> | |||
As a comparison, schizophrenia is estimated to be at least 70% heritable, of which 30% of the actual genes have been accounted for.<ref></ref><ref>Jianxin S, et al. Common variants on chromosome 6p22.1 are associated with schizophrenia. July 1, 2009, Nature</ref><ref>Stefansson H, et al. Common variants conferring risk of schizophrenia. July 1, 2009, Nature</ref><ref>Purcell SM, et al. Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia that overlaps with bipolar disorder. July 1, 2009, Nature</ref> | |||
A 2003 study found that two variables—] and the degree of educational attainment of children's fathers—partially explained the black–white gap in cognitive ability test scores, undermining the hereditarian view that they stemmed from immutable genetic factors.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=McKay |first1=Patrick F. |last2=Doverspike |first2=Dennis |last3=Bowen-Hilton |first3=Doreen |last4=McKay |first4=Quintonia D. |title=The Effects of Demographic Variables and Stereotype Threat on Black/White Differences in Cognitive Ability Test Performance |journal=Journal of Business and Psychology |date=2003 |volume=18 |issue=1 |pages=1–14 |doi=10.1023/A:1025062703113 |s2cid=142317051}}</ref> | |||
=== Genetic hypothesis === | |||
] (SES) varies both between and within populations, but Black-White differences in IQ persist among the children of parents matched for SES, and the gap is largest among the children of wealthiest and best educated parents.<ref>Reviewed in Neisser et al. (1996). Data from the ] as reported in figure adapted from Herrnstein and Murray (1994), p. 288.</ref>]] | |||
Much of the research on explaining group differences stems from an observation promoted first by Arthur Jensen and later James Flynn and others regarding an environmental explanation for group differences. According to Jensen<ref>Jensen (1998) The g Factor</ref> the very high within-group ] of IQ (within both white and black populations) presents a problem for environmental explanations of group differences in IQ. They consider two general classes of environmental factors: common environmental factors and X-factors. Common environmental factors vary within and between populations. X-factors vary between populations, but do not vary substantially within populations. They first consider common environmental factors. To account for a 1 SD B-W IQ gap only in terms of common environmental factors would require very large environmental differences. For example, if the within-group heritability of IQ is 80%, then a B-W IQ difference of 2.24 SD in common environmental factors is required. For a heritability of 40%, a difference of 1.29 SD is required. Jensen and Flynn agree that it is an empirical question whether common environmental factors that influence IQ differ between whites and blacks to such an extent, and both agree that most commonly suggested environmental factors do not. Jensen believes that empirical evidence supports the view that the B-W IQ gap is caused by both common environmental factors and genetic factors. Flynn disagrees and believes that empirical evidence supports the view that the B-W IQ gap is caused by yet unrecognized environmental factors.<ref>Flynn (1980) and Flynn (1999)</ref> | |||
===Socioeconomic environment=== | |||
The alternative to common environmental factors is to hypothesize that X-factors account for the B-W IQ gap. A frequently-cited example from Lewontin describes the effect of a hypothetical X-factor. Imagine that the height of "ordinary genetically varied corn" is 100% heritable when grown in a uniform environment. Further imagine that two populations of corn are grown: one in a normal nutrient environment and the other in a deficient nutrient environment. Consequently, the average height of the corn grown in the deficient nutrient environment is less than the average height of the corn grown in the normal environment. In such a scenario, the within-group heritability of height is 100% in both populations, but the substantial difference between groups are due entirely to environmental factors. The quality of the nutrient is an "X-factor" in the language of Jensen and Flynn. With respect to the B-W IQ gap, Jensen suggests that effects associated with racism (both overt and ]) might be X-factors. Flynn believes that attributing the B-W gap to the effects of racism is incorrect, because the most plausible ways in which discrimination could affect IQ are themselves common environmental factors. These may include psychological effects such as ]; biological effects such as poor nutrition, health care and living close to toxic environments; and educational effects such as a lack of good schools. Instead, Flynn and his colleague William Dickens have developed more complicated models to explain the black-white gap in terms of environmental factors. One initial motivation of the Dickens-Flynn theory was Flynn's observation that IQ test scores have been rising over time in countries around the world – termed the ]. Flynn and others believe an explanation for the Flynn effect may elucidate the cause of the B-W gap. Jensen and others disagree. | |||
Different aspects of the socioeconomic environment in which children are raised have been shown to correlate with part of the IQ gap, but they do not account for the entire gap.{{sfn|Hunt|2010|page=428}} According to a 2006 review, these factors account for slightly less than half of one standard deviation.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Magnuson |first1=Katherine A. |last2=Duncan |first2=Greg J. |title=The role of family socioeconomic resources in the black–white test score gap among young children |journal=] |date=December 2006 |volume=26 |issue=4 |pages=365–399 |doi=10.1016/j.dr.2006.06.004}}</ref> | |||
Other research has focused on different causes of variation within low socioeconomic status (SES) and high SES groups.<ref name="Scarr-Salapatek1971">{{cite journal |last1=Scarr-Salapatek |first1=S. |year=1971 |title=Race, social class, and IQ. |journal=Science |volume=174 |issue=4016 |pages=1285–95 |doi=10.1126/science.174.4016.1285 |pmid=5167501 |bibcode=1971Sci...174.1285S}}</ref><ref name="Scarr-Salapatek1974">{{cite journal |last1=Scarr-Salapatek |first1=S. |year=1974 |title=Some myths about heritability and IQ. |doi=10.1038/251463b0 |journal=Nature |volume=251 |issue=5475 |pages=463–464 |bibcode=1974Natur.251..463S |s2cid=32437709 |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name="Rowe1994">D. C. Rowe. (1994). ''The Limits of Family Influence: Genes, Experience and Behaviour''. Guilford Press. London</ref> | |||
A few of the notable proponents of the partly genetic hypothesis are ], ] and ]. | |||
In the US, among low SES groups, genetic differences account for a smaller proportion of the variance in IQ than among high SES populations.<ref name="Kirkpatrick2015">{{cite journal |last1=Kirkpatrick |first1=R. M. |last2=McGue |first2=M. |last3=Iacono |first3=W. G. |year=2015 |title=Replication of a gene-environment interaction Via Multimodel inference: additive-genetic variance in adolescents' general cognitive ability increases with family-of-origin socioeconomic status |doi=10.1007/s10519-014-9698-y |journal=Behav Genet |volume=45 |issue=2 |pages=200–14 |pmc=4374354 |pmid=25539975}}</ref> Such effects are predicted by the '']'' hypothesis—that genotypes are transformed into phenotypes through nonadditive synergistic effects of the environment.<ref>{{Cite journal |title=Nature-nuture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. |journal=Psychological Review |pages=568–586 |volume=101 |issue=4 |doi=10.1037/0033-295x.101.4.568 |first1=Urie |last1=Bronfenbrenner |first2=Stephen J. |last2=Ceci |pmid=7984707 |date=October 1994|s2cid=17402964 }}</ref> {{harvp|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}} suggest that high SES individuals are more likely to be able to develop their full biological potential, whereas low SES individuals are likely to be hindered in their development by adverse environmental conditions. The same review also points out that adoption studies generally are biased towards including only high and high middle SES adoptive families, meaning that they will tend to overestimate average genetic effects. They also note that studies of adoption from lower-class homes to middle-class homes have shown that such children experience a 12 to 18 point gain in IQ relative to children who remain in low SES homes.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}} A 2015 study found that environmental factors (namely, family income, maternal education, maternal verbal ability/knowledge, learning materials in the home, parenting factors, child birth order, and child birth weight) accounted for the black–white gap in cognitive ability test scores.{{sfn|Cottrell|Newman|Roisman|2015}} | |||
===Test bias=== | |||
] and ] examined 10 categories of research evidence from around the world to contrast "a hereditarian model" (50% genetic-50% cultural) and a culture-only model (0% genetic-100% cultural). Their article "''Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability''" was published in the ] journal ''Psychology, Public Policy and Law'' showing evidence that they believe supports the hereditarian model.<ref>http://psychology.uwo.ca/faculty/rushtonpdfs/PPPL1.pdf Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability</ref><ref>http://www.innovations-report.de/html/berichte/studien/bericht-43536.html Black-White-East Asian IQ differences at least 50% genetic, major law review journal concludes</ref> Rushton and Jensen (2005a) believe that the best explanation for the gap is that 50%-80% of the group differences in average US IQ is genetic.<ref>Rushton and Jensen (2005a), cited in "", and Murray (2005)</ref> | |||
A number of studies have reached the conclusion that IQ tests may be biased against certain groups.<ref>{{harvnb|Cronshaw et al.|2006|p=278}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Verney et al.|2005}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Borsboom|2006}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Shuttleworth-Edwards et al.|2004}}</ref> The validity and reliability of IQ scores obtained from outside the United States and Europe have been questioned, in part because of the inherent difficulty of comparing IQ scores between cultures.<ref name="Richardson 2004">{{harvnb|Richardson|2004}}</ref><ref name="Hunt & Wittmann 2008">{{harvnb|Hunt|Wittmann|2008}}</ref> Several researchers have argued that cultural differences limit the appropriateness of standard IQ tests in non-industrialized communities.<ref>{{harvnb|Irvine|1983}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Irvine|Berry|1988}} a collection of articles by several authors discussing the limits of assessment by intelligence tests in different communities in the world. In particular, {{harvp|Reuning|1988}} describes the difficulties in devising and administering tests for Kalahari bushmen.</ref> | |||
A 1996 report by the ] states that intelligence can be difficult to compare across cultures, and notes that differing familiarity with test materials can produce substantial differences in test results; it also says that tests are accurate predictors of future achievement for black and white Americans, and are in that sense unbiased.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} The view that tests accurately predict future educational attainment is reinforced by ] in his 1998 book ''IQ and Human Intelligence'',<ref>{{harvnb|Mackintosh|1998|p=174}}: "Despite widespread belief to the contrary, however, there is ample evidence, both in Britain and the USA, that IQ tests predict educational attainment just about as well in ethnic minorities as in the white majority."</ref> and by a 1999 literature review by {{harvp|Brown|Reynolds|Whitaker|1999}}. | |||
Other evidence, such as the ], certain racial admixture studies, behavior genetic modeling of group differences, "life-history" traits, and evolutionary explanations have also been proposed to indicate a genetic contribution to the IQ gaps and explain how these arose.<ref>Reviewed by Rushton and Jensen (2005).</ref> | |||
James R. Flynn, surveying studies on the topic, notes that the weight and presence of many test questions depends on what sorts of information and modes of thinking are culturally valued.<ref name="FlynnIntelligence">{{cite journal |journal=Intelligence |issue=70 |pages=73–83 |year=2018 |url=https://scottbarrykaufman.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/08/1-s2.0-S0160289618300904-main.pdf |title=Reflections about intelligence over 40 years |access-date=2019-02-02 |archive-date=2019-02-03 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190203030438/https://scottbarrykaufman.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/08/1-s2.0-S0160289618300904-main.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref> | |||
The decoding of the ] has enabled scientists to search for sections of the genome that contribute to cognitive abilities. Current studies using ] have yielded little success in the search for genes influencing intelligence. ] is confident that QTLs responsible for the variation in IQ scores exist, but that more powerful tools of analysis will be required to detect them.<ref>{{cite journal|title=The quest for quantitative trait loci associated with intelligence|doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.01.001|last=Plomin|authorlink=Robert Plomin|year=2005|journal=Intelligence|volume=34|pages=513}}</ref> | |||
===Stereotype threat and minority status=== | |||
A 2005 literature review article on the links between race and intelligence in ] stated that no gene has been shown to be linked to intelligence, "so attempts to provide a compelling genetic link of race to intelligence are not feasible at this time".<ref name="Intelligence, Race, and Genetics"/> However, in 2007 the ] reported preliminary results suggesting that some genes which influence IQ may be distributed unequally between races.<ref>Amy Harmon. ]. November 17, 2007.</ref> Some researchers have expressed reluctance to investigate possible links between genes and intelligence, due to the controversy it can produce.<ref>Antonio Regalado. ]. June 16, 2006.</ref> | |||
{{Main|Stereotype threat}} | |||
] is the fear that one's behavior will confirm an existing ] of a group with which one identifies or by which one is defined; this fear may in turn lead to an impairment of performance.<ref>{{harvnb|Aronson|Wilson|Akert| 2005}}</ref> Testing situations that highlight the fact that intelligence is being measured tend to lower the scores of individuals from racial-ethnic groups who already score lower on average or are expected to score lower. Stereotype threat conditions cause larger than expected IQ differences among groups.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Steele |first1=Claude M. |title=A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance |journal=American Psychologist |volume=52 |issue=6 |year=1997 |pages=613–629 |issn=0003-066X |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613 |pmid=9174398 |citeseerx=10.1.1.319.8283|s2cid=19952 }}</ref> Psychometrician ] considers that there is little doubt that the effects of stereotype threat contribute to the IQ gap between black people and white people.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|p=348}} | |||
A large number of studies have shown that systemically disadvantaged minorities, such as the African American minority of the United States, generally perform worse in the educational system and in intelligence tests than the majority groups or less disadvantaged minorities such as immigrant or "voluntary" minorities.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} The explanation of these findings may be that children of caste-like minorities, due to the systemic limitations of their prospects of social advancement, do not have "]", i.e. they do not have the confidence that acquiring the skills valued by majority society, such as those skills measured by IQ tests, is worthwhile. They may even deliberately reject certain behaviors that are seen as "]."{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}}{{sfn|Ogbu|1978}}{{sfn|Ogbu|1994}} Research published in 1997 indicates that part of the black–white gap in cognitive ability test scores is due to racial differences in test motivation.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Chan |first1=D. |last2=Schmitt |first2=N. |last3=DeShon |first3=R. P. |last4=Clause |first4=C. S. |last5=Delbridge |first5=K. |date=April 1997 |title=Reactions to cognitive ability tests: the relationships between race, test performance, face validity perceptions, and test-taking motivation |journal=The Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=82 |issue=2 |pages=300–310 |issn=0021-9010 |pmid=9109288 |doi=10.1037/0021-9010.82.2.300|url=https://ink.library.smu.edu.sg/soss_research/230 }}</ref> | |||
=== Environmental effects === | |||
Critics of the significantly genetic view, such as ], argue that these studies are either flawed and thus inconclusive, or else that they support a primarily environment (<20% genetic) hypothesis.<ref>For example: Nisbett (2005), Suzuki and Aronson (2005), Sternberg (2005), Dickens (2005)</ref> For example, Dolan and Hamaker (2001) argue that the statistical methods linking the Black-White gap to ''g'' are insufficient. They reanalyzed the data from several earlier studies and concluded that Spearman's hypothesis is not an "empirically established fact" (i.e., that Black-White IQ differences may be due to differences in common factors other than ''g'') due to insufficient power in the data to choose between alternative models. "This leaves the validity of Spearman's hypothesis, considered a central justification for the genetic explanation, an unresolved question." However, they did confirm that the Black-White IQ gap is not due to measurement artifacts, and is instead due to some measured factor that varies both within and between groups. | |||
Some researchers have suggested that stereotype threat should not be interpreted as a factor in real-life performance gaps, and have raised the possibility of ].<ref name="Ganley2013">{{cite journal |vauthors=Ganley CM, Mingle LA, Ryan AM, Ryan K, Vasilyeva M, Perry M |title=An examination of stereotype threat effects on girls' mathematics performance |journal=Developmental Psychology |volume=49 |issue=10 |pages=1886–97 |date=October 2013 |pmid=23356523 |doi=10.1037/a0031412 |url=https://dl.dropboxusercontent.com/u/85192141/2013-ganley.pdf |citeseerx=10.1.1.353.4436 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20140719005546/https://dl.dropboxusercontent.com/u/85192141/2013-ganley.pdf |archive-date=19 July 2014}}</ref><ref name="Stoet2012">{{Cite journal |vauthors=Stoet G, Geary DC |doi=10.1037/a0026617 |title=Can stereotype threat explain the gender gap in mathematics performance and achievement? |journal=Review of General Psychology |volume=16 |pages=93–102 |year=2012 |s2cid=145724069}} {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160112130459/http://volition.gla.ac.uk/~stoet/pdf/Stoet-Geary-RGP2012.pdf |date=2016-01-12 }}</ref><ref name="Flore2014">{{cite journal |vauthors=Flore PC, Wicherts JM |title=Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis |journal=Journal of School Psychology |volume=53 |issue=1 |pages=25–44 |date=February 2015 |pmid=25636259 |doi=10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002|s2cid=206516995 }}</ref> Other critics have focused on correcting what they claim are misconceptions of early studies showing a large effect.<ref name="Sackett2004a">{{cite journal |vauthors=Sackett PR, Hardison CM, Cullen MJ |title=On interpreting stereotype threat as accounting for African American-White differences on cognitive tests |journal=The American Psychologist |volume=59 |issue=1 |pages=7–13 |date=January 2004 |pmid=14736315 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.7 |url=http://www2.uni-jena.de/svw/igc/studies/ss03/sackitt_hardison_cullen_2004.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130404150510/http://www2.uni-jena.de/svw/igc/studies/ss03/sackitt_hardison_cullen_2004.pdf |archive-date=2013-04-04}}</ref> However, numerous ] and systematic reviews have shown significant evidence for the effects of stereotype threat, though the phenomenon defies over-simplistic characterization.<ref name="Pennington-2016">{{cite journal |vauthors=Pennington CR, Heim D, Levy AR, Larkin DT |date=2016-01-11 |title=Twenty Years of Stereotype Threat Research: A Review of Psychological Mediators |journal=PLOS ONE |volume=11 |issue=1 |pages=e0146487 |bibcode=2016PLoSO..1146487P |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0146487 |pmc=4713435 |pmid=26752551 |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name="Nguyen-2008">{{cite journal |vauthors=Nguyen HH, Ryan AM |date=November 2008 |title=Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence |journal=The Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=93 |issue=6 |pages=1314–34 |doi=10.1037/a0012702 |pmid=19025250|s2cid=36769821 }}</ref><ref name="Walton-2009">{{Cite journal |last1=Walton |first1=Gregory M. |last2=Spencer |first2=Steven J. |date=2009-09-01 |title=Latent Ability: Grades and Test Scores Systematically Underestimate the Intellectual Ability of Negatively Stereotyped Students |journal=Psychological Science |volume=20 |issue=9 |pages=1132–1139 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02417.x |issn=0956-7976 |pmid=19656335 |s2cid=25810191|doi-access=free }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Gentile |first1=Ambra |last2=Boca |first2=Stefano |last3=Giammusso |first3=Isabella |date=2018-11-01 |title='You play like a Woman!' Effects of gender stereotype threat on Women's performance in physical and sport activities: A meta-analysis |journal=Psychology of Sport and Exercise |volume=39 |pages=95–103 |doi=10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.07.013 |s2cid=149490634 |issn=1469-0292}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Lamont |first1=Ruth A. |last2=Swift |first2=Hannah J. |last3=Abrams |first3=Dominic |year=2015 |title=A Review and Meta-Analysis of Age-Based Stereotype Threat: Negative Stereotypes, Not Facts, Do the Damage. |journal=Psychology and Aging |volume=30 |issue=1 |pages=180–193 |doi=10.1037/a0038586 |issn=1939-1498 |pmc=4360754 |pmid=25621742}}</ref><ref name="Picho-2013">{{Cite journal |last1=Picho |first1=Katherine |last2=Rodriguez |first2=Ariel |last3=Finnie |first3=Lauren |date=May 2013 |title=Exploring the Moderating Role of Context on the Mathematics Performance of Females Under Stereotype Threat: A Meta-Analysis |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/237000996 |journal=The Journal of Social Psychology |volume=153 |issue=3 |pages=299–333 |doi=10.1080/00224545.2012.737380 |pmid=23724702 |s2cid=45950675}}</ref><ref name="Liu-2020">{{Cite journal |last1=Liu |first1=Songqi |last2=Liu |first2=Pei |last3=Wang |first3=Mo |last4=Zhang |first4=Baoshan |date=July 2020 |title=Effectiveness of Stereotype Threat Interventions: A Meta-Analytic Review |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/343149798 |journal=Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=currently in press |issue=6 |pages=921–949 |doi=10.1037/apl0000770 |pmid=32772526 |s2cid=221098319}}</ref>{{excessive citations inline|date=June 2024}} For instance, one meta-analysis found that with female subjects "subtle threat-activating cues produced the largest effect, followed by blatant and moderately explicit cues" while with minorities "moderately explicit stereotype threat-activating cues produced the largest effect, followed by blatant and subtle cues".<ref name="Nguyen-2008" /> | |||
Nisbett (2005) argues that many studies find results that do not support the genetic hypothesis. They include studies on IQ and skin color that reported that the average correlation between skin color and IQ is .1 (the average correlation between IQ and judged “Negroidness” of features is even lower); IQ and self-reported European ancestry; IQ and blood groups showing degree of European Ancestry; IQ among children in post WWII Germany born to black and white American soldiers; and IQ among mixed-race children born to either a black or a white mother. He argues that these are direct tests of the genetic hypothesis and of more value than indirect variables, such as skull size and reaction time. He argues that "There is not a shred of evidence in this literature, which draws on studies having a total of five very different designs, that the gap has a genetic basis." He argues further that many intervention and adoption studies also find results that do not support the genetic hypothesis. He also argues "that the Black-White IQ gap has lessened considerably in recent decades."<ref> Richard E. Nisbett, Psychology, Public Policy, and Law: June 2005 Vol. 11, No. 2, 302-310</ref> Hunt and Carlson<ref name="autogenerated2" /> argue that Nisbett's interpretations are far too strong in light of problems with these studies that have been recognized for decades.<ref>Loehlin, J.C., Lindzey, G., & Spuhler, J. (1975). Racial Differences in Intelligence. San Francisco: Freeman.</ref> Gottfredson writes that the studies Nisbett cites "actually lack the ability to rule out any hypothesis at all, genetic or not".<ref>http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/2007doublestandards.pdf</ref> | |||
Some researchers have argued that studies of stereotype threat may in fact systematically under-represent its effects, since such studies measure "only that portion of psychological threat that research has identified and remedied. To the extent that unidentified or unremedied psychological threats further undermine performance, the results underestimate the bias."<ref name="Walton-2009"/> | |||
Dickens (2005) states that "Although the direct evidence on the role of environment is not definitive, it mostly suggests that genetic differences are not necessary to explain racial differences. Advocates of the hereditarian position have therefore turned to indirect evidence...The indirect evidence on the role of genes in explaining the black-white gap does not tell us how much of the gap genes explain and may be of no value at all in deciding whether genes do play a role. Because the direct evidence on ancestry, adoption, and cross-fostering is most consistent with little or no role for genes, it is unlikely that the black-white gap has a large genetic component."<ref> Dickens, William T. The Future of Children - Volume 15, Number 1, Spring 2005, pp. 55-69</ref> | |||
==Research into possible genetic factors== | |||
] and ] (2006), with data from "the first large, nationally representative sample" of its kind, report finding only a very small racial difference when measuring mental function for children aged eight to twelve months, and that even these differences disappear when including a "limited set of controls". "On tests of intelligence, Blacks systematically score worse than Whites, whereas Asians frequently outperform Whites. Some have argued that genetic differences across races account for the gap. Using a newly available nationally representative data set that includes a test of mental function for children aged eight to twelve months, we find only minor racial differences in test outcomes (0.06 standard deviation units in the raw data) between Blacks and Whites that disappear with the inclusion of a limited set of controls. The only statistically significant racial difference is that Asian children score slightly worse than those of other races." They argue that their report poses "a substantial challenge to the simplest, most direct, and most often articulated genetic stories regarding racial differences in mental function." They conclude that "to the extent that there are any genetically-driven racial differences in intelligence, these gaps must either emerge after the age of one, or operate along dimensions not captured by this early test of mental cognition."<ref>Roland G. Fryer Jr. and Steven D. Levitt, "," ''The Review of Economics and Statistics'' 86, no. 2 (2004). </ref> | |||
{{see also|Heritability of IQ}} | |||
Although IQ differences between individuals have been shown to have a large hereditary component, it does not follow that mean group-level disparities (between-group differences) in IQ necessarily have a genetic basis.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |last3=Blair |first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens |first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |date=2012 |title=Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments. |journal=American Psychologist |language=en |volume=67 |issue=2 |pages=130–159 |doi=10.1037/a0026699 |issn=1935-990X |pmid=22233090}}</ref><ref name="Nisbett-2012" /> The scientific consensus is that there is no evidence for a genetic component behind IQ differences between racial groups.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Bird |first1=Kevin |last2=Jackson |first2=John P. |last3=Winston |first3=Andrew S. |date=2024 |title=Confronting Scientific Racism in Psychology: Lessons from Evolutionary Biology and Genetics |url=https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Famp0001228 |journal=American Psychologist |volume=79 |issue=4 |pages=497–508 |doi=10.1037/amp0001228 |pmid=39037836 |quote=Recent articles claim that the folk categories of race are genetically meaningful divisions, and that evolved genetic differences among races and nations are important for explaining immutable differences in cognitive ability, educational attainment, crime, sexual behavior, and wealth; all claims that are opposed by a strong scientific consensus to the contrary.}}</ref>{{sfn|Ceci|Williams|2009|pages=788–789, "There is an emerging consensus about racial and gender equality in genetic determinants of intelligence; most researchers, including ourselves, agree that genes do not explain between-group differences"}}{{sfn|Hunt|2010|page=447|ps= , "It is worth remembering that no genes related to difference in cognitive skills across the various racial and ethnic groups have ever been discovered. The argument for genetic differences has been carried forward largely by circumstantial evidence. Of course, tomorrow afternoon genetic mechanisms producing racial and ethnic differences in intelligence might be discovered, but there have been a lot of investigations, and tomorrow has not come for quite some time now."}}{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011||pages=334–338, 344}}<ref name="Nisbett-2012">{{cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |last3=Blair |first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens |first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |author-link5=Jim Flynn (academic) |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |author-link6=Diane F. Halpern |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |date=2012 |title=Group differences in IQ are best understood as environmental in origin |journal=American Psychologist |volume=67 |number=6 |pages=503–504 |doi=10.1037/a0029772 |issn=0003-066X |pmid=22963427 |author-link1=Richard E. Nisbett}}</ref><ref name="Kaplan-2015">{{Cite journal |last=Kaplan |first=Jonathan Michael |date=January 2015 |title=Race, IQ, and the search for statistical signals associated with so-called "X"-factors: environments, racism, and the "hereditarian hypothesis" |journal=Biology & Philosophy |language=en |volume=30 |issue=1 |pages=1–17 |doi=10.1007/s10539-014-9428-0 |s2cid=85351431 |issn=0169-3867}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Panofsky |first1=Aaron |last2=Dasgupta |first2=Kushan |last3=Iturriaga |first3=Nicole |title=How White nationalists mobilize genetics: From genetic ancestry and human biodiversity to counterscience and metapolitics |journal=American Journal of Physical Anthropology |year=2021 |volume=175 |issue=2 |pages=387–398 |doi=10.1002/ajpa.24150 |issn=0002-9483 |pmid=32986847 |pmc=9909835 |quote=he claims that genetics defines racial groups and makes them different, that IQ and cultural differences among racial groups are caused by genes, and that racial inequalities within and between nations are the inevitable outcome of long evolutionary processes are neither new nor supported by science (either old or new). |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name="LewontinSameTitle">{{cite journal |last1=Lewontin |first1=Richard C. |title=Race and Intelligence |journal=Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists |date=March 1970 |volume=26 |issue=3 |pages=2–8 |doi=10.1080/00963402.1970.11457774 |bibcode=1970BuAtS..26c...2L |url=https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00963402.1970.11457774 |access-date=26 April 2021 |archive-date=10 June 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210610120351/https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00963402.1970.11457774 |url-status=live }}</ref>{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} Growing evidence indicates that environmental factors, not genetic ones, explain the racial IQ gap.{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}}<ref name="Nisbett-2012" />{{sfn|Nevid|2014|page=271}}<ref name="Kaplan-2015" /> | |||
=== |
===Genetics of race and intelligence=== | ||
{{main|Race and genetics}} | |||
The secular, international increase in test scores, commonly called the ], is seen by Flynn and others as reason to expect the eventual convergence of average black and white IQ scores. Flynn argues that the average IQ scores in several countries have increased about 3 points per decade during the 20th century, which he and others attribute predominantly to environmental causes.<ref>Flynn 1987, Flynn 1987b, Flynn 1999, Flynn 1999b</ref> This means, given the same test, the mean performance of ] today could be higher than the mean for ]s in 1920, though the gains causing this appear to have occurred predominantly in the lower half of the IQ distribution.<ref>Colom et al. 2005</ref> If an unknown environmental factor can cause changes in IQ over time, they argue, then contemporary differences between groups could also be due to an unknown environmental factor. One unresolved question is whether the secular IQ gains can be predominantly a real change in cognitive ability. Flynn's face-value answer to this question is "No",<ref>Flynn 1999</ref> and some other researchers have found reason to concur. In terms of the mixture of ability factors that IQ tests were designed to measure, such as ''g'' and verbal and mathematical ability, changes in IQ scores over time are different than either within-group individual differences and between group differences.<ref>Flynn (2007)</ref><ref> Wicherts et al. (2004) wrote that "the gains cannot be explained solely by increases at the level of the latent variables (common factors), which IQ tests purport to measure". In other words, according to this study, some of the inter-generational difference in IQ is attributable to bias or other artifacts, and not real gains in ] or higher-order ability factors.</ref> For example, there has been little increase over time in performance on either the forward digit-span or reverse digit-span subtests, and tests of school achievement have been less affected than tests of abstract reasoning.<ref>James Robert Flynn (2007) ''What is Intelligence?: Beyond the Flynn Effect''. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge</ref> Other recent studies have found that ''g'' has improved substantially.<ref>Te Nijenhuis Jan; De Jong Mart-Jan; Evers Arne; Van Der Flier Henk; European journal of personality (Eur. j. pers.) 2004, vol. 18, no5, pp. 405-434</ref><ref> Roberto Colom and Oscar Garcia-Lopez, Journal of Biosocial Science (2003), 35: 33-39 Cambridge University Press</ref> ] size has increased and the shape changed during the last 150 years in the US; these changes must occur by early childhood because of the early development of the vault.<ref>"<cite>Changes in vault dimensions must occur by early childhood because of the early development of the vault.</cite>" | |||
Geneticist ] argued that the question about the possible genetic effects on the test score gap is muddled by the general focus on "race" rather than on populations defined by gene frequency or by geographical proximity, and by the general insistence on phrasing the question in terms of heritability.<ref name="Templeton 2001">{{harvnb|Templeton|2001}}</ref> Templeton pointed out that racial groups neither represent ] nor distinct ], and that therefore there is no basis for making claims about the general intelligence of races.<ref name="Templeton 2001"/> He argued that, for these reasons, the search for possible genetic influences on the black–white test score gap is ''a priori'' flawed, because there is no genetic material shared by all Africans or by all Europeans. {{harvp|Mackintosh|2011}}, on the other hand, argued that by using genetic cluster analysis to correlate gene frequencies with continental populations it might be possible to show that African populations have a higher frequency of certain genetic variants that contribute to differences in average intelligence. Such a hypothetical situation could hold without all Africans carrying the same genes or belonging to a single evolutionary lineage. According to Mackintosh, a biological basis for the observed gap in IQ test performance thus cannot be ruled out on ''a priori'' grounds.{{Page needed|date=January 2022}} | |||
"<cite>During the 125 years under consideration, cranial vaults have become markedly higher, somewhat narrower, with narrower faces. The changes in cranial morphology are probably in large part due to changes in growth at the cranial base due to improved environmental conditions. The changes are likely a combination of phenotypic plasticity and genetic changes over this period.</cite>" </ref> It also estimated that the average white IQ in past decades was lower than current average black IQs, a fact which shows the change of IQ over time.<ref></ref> But while black Americans in the early 21st century may score higher than white Americans in the early 19th century, the fact remains that the roughly 1 standard deviation IQ gap between black and white Americans living at the same time has held constant since the earliest days of intelligence testing, convincing some observers like Richard Lynn that the black-white gap in the U.S. is not only genetic, but 100% genetic. Indeed Arthur Jensen is struck by how consistent the gap has been, despite the enormous rise in the scores of both races. | |||
{{harvtxt|Hunt|2010|page=447}} noted that "no genes related to difference in cognitive skills have across the various racial and ethnic groups have ever been discovered. The argument for genetic differences has been carried forward largely by circumstantial evidence. Of course, tomorrow afternoon genetic mechanisms producing racial and ethnic differences in intelligence might be discovered, but there have been a lot of investigations, and tomorrow has not come for quite some time now." {{harvtxt|Mackintosh|2011|page=344}} concurred, noting that while several environmental factors have been shown to influence the IQ gap, the evidence for a genetic influence has been negligible. A 2012 review by {{harvp|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair| Dickens|2012a}} concluded that the entire IQ gap can be explained by known environmental factors, and Mackintosh found this view to be plausible. | |||
==== Health ==== | |||
]s ''at least'' 10 µg/dL. Black and Hispanic children have measurably higher levels than white children. High levels of lead at an early age may affect intelligence.]] | |||
More recent research attempting to identify genetic loci associated with individual-level differences in IQ has yielded promising results, which led the editorial board of '']'' to issue a statement differentiating this research from the "racist" pseudoscience which it acknowledged has dogged intelligence research since its inception.<ref name="Nature-2017" /> It characterized the idea of genetically determined differences in intelligence between races as definitively false.<ref name="Nature-2017">{{Cite journal |date=25 May 2017 |title=Intelligence research should not be held back by its past |journal=Nature |volume=545 |issue=7655 |pages=385–386 |doi=10.1038/nature.2017.22021 |pmid=28541341 |bibcode=2017Natur.545R.385. |s2cid=4449918|doi-access=free }}</ref> Analysis of polygenic scores sampled from the 1000 Genomes Project has likewise found no evidence that intelligence was under diversifying selection in Africans and Europeans, suggesting that genetic differences make up a negligible component of the observed Black-White gap in IQ.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Bird |first=Kevin A. |date=2 February 2021 |title=No support for the hereditarian hypothesis of the Black–White achievement gap using polygenic scores and tests for divergent selection |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ajpa.24216 |journal=] |language=en |volume=175 |issue=2 |pages=465–476 |doi=10.1002/ajpa.24216 |pmid=33529393 |issn=0002-9483 |access-date=1 November 2024 |via=Wiley Online Library}}</ref> | |||
{{main|Health and intelligence}} | |||
{{main|Race and health}} | |||
Numerous explanations beside genetics have been proposed to account for the IQ gaps in the U.S.<ref>Joel Wiesen, "," Applied Personnel Research, March 18, 2005.</ref> | |||
High rates of low birth-weight babies, lower rates of breastfeeding, and exposure to toxins are some factors. The ] is often cited as evidence that average IQ scores have changed greatly and rapidly, for reasons poorly understood, thus the IQ gap between races could change in the future or is changing, especially if the Flynn effect started earlier for Whites. | |||
===Heritability within and between groups=== | |||
High levels of lead at an early age may affect intelligence; studies indicate that black and Hispanic children have measurably higher levels than white children. A 10 µg/dL increase in blood lead at 24 months of age is associated with a 5.8-point lower IQ later in life.<ref> David C. Bellinger PhD, MSc1, Karen M. Stiles PhD, MN1, and Herbert L. Needleman MD1. Pediatrics Vol. 90 No. 6 December 1992, pp. 855-861</ref> In 1976 77.8% of all children had ''at least'' this much lead in their blood.<ref> CDC.</ref> | |||
].|330x330px]] | |||
] of intelligence have reported high heritability values. However, these studies have been criticized for being based on questionable assumptions.<ref>{{Cite book |last1=Carson |first1=Michael |title='Race', IQ and Genes |last2=Beckwith |first2=Jon |date=2001 |publisher=John Wiley & Sons, Ltd |isbn=978-0-470-01590-2 |pages=1–5 |language=en |doi=10.1002/9780470015902.a0005689.pub3}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Beckwith |first1=Jon |last2=Morris |first2=Corey A. |date=December 2008 |title=Twin Studies of Political Behavior: Untenable Assumptions? |journal=Perspectives on Politics |language=en |volume=6 |issue=4 |pages=785–791 |doi=10.1017/S1537592708081917 |s2cid=55630117 |issn=1541-0986 }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Kamin |first1=Leon J. |last2=Goldberger |first2=Arthur S. |date=February 2002 |title=Twin Studies in Behavioral Research: A Skeptical View |journal=Theoretical Population Biology |volume=61 |issue=1 |pages=83–95 |doi=10.1006/tpbi.2001.1555 |pmid=11895384 |bibcode=2002TPBio..61...83K |issn=0040-5809}}</ref> When used in the context of human ], the term "heritability" can be misleading, as it does not necessarily convey information about the relative importance of genetic or environmental factors on the development of a given trait, nor does it convey the extent to which that trait is genetically determined.{{sfn|Moore|Shenk|2016}} Arguments in support of a genetic explanation of racial differences in IQ are sometimes fallacious. For instance, hereditarians have sometimes cited the failure of known environmental factors to account for such differences, or the high heritability of intelligence within races, as evidence that racial differences in IQ are genetic.<ref>{{harvnb|Mackenzie|1984}}</ref> | |||
Exposure to lead is frequently attributed to housing conditions including lead based paint, which is no longer used but has accumulated in older buildings; people of lower economic means are more frequently exposed to lead from housing.<ref> NAACP Press Release 17 July 01</ref> | |||
Psychometricians have found that intelligence is substantially heritable within populations, with 30–50% of variance in IQ scores in early childhood being attributable to genetic factors in analyzed US populations, increasing to 75–80% by late adolescence.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}}<ref name="Deary, Johnson & Houlihan 2009">{{harvnb|Deary|Johnson|Houlihan|2009}}</ref> In biology heritability is defined as the ratio of variation attributable to genetic differences in an observable ] to the trait's total observable variation. The heritability of a trait describes the proportion of variation in the trait that is attributable to genetic factors within a particular population. A heritability of 1 indicates that variation correlates fully with genetic variation and a heritability of 0 indicates that there is no correlation between the trait and genes at all. In psychological testing, heritability tends to be understood as the degree of correlation between the results of a test taker and those of their biological parents. However, since high heritability is simply a correlation between child and parents, it does not describe the causes of heritability which in humans can be either genetic or environmental. | |||
====Stereotype threat==== | |||
{{main|Stereotype threat}} | |||
Therefore, a high heritability measure does not imply that a trait is genetic or unchangeable. In addition, environmental factors that affect all group members equally will not be measured by heritability, and the heritability of a trait may also change over time in response to changes in the distribution of genetic and environmental factors.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} High heritability does not imply that all of the heritability is genetically determined; rather, it can also be due to environmental differences that affect only a certain genetically defined group (indirect heritability).<ref name="Block 2002">{{harvnb|Block|2002}}</ref> | |||
] threat is the fear that one's behavior will confirm an existing stereotype of a group with which one identifies; this fear may in turn lead to an impairment of performance.<ref>Aronson, Wilson, & Akert, 2005</ref> Testing situations that highlight the fact that intelligence is being measured tend to lower the scores of individuals from racial-ethnic groups that already score lower on average. Stereotype threat conditions cause larger than expected IQ differences among groups but do not fully explain the gaps found in non-threatening test conditions. | |||
The figure to the right demonstrates how heritability works. In each of the two gardens the difference between tall and short cornstalks is 100% heritable, as cornstalks that are genetically disposed for growing tall will become taller than those without this disposition. But the difference in height between the cornstalks to the left and those on the right is 100% environmental, as it is due to different nutrients being supplied to the two gardens. Hence, the causes of differences within a group and between groups may not be the same, even when looking at traits that are highly heritable.<ref name="Block 2002" /> | |||
====Quality of education==== | |||
Some researchers have written that studies that find test performance gaps between races even after adjusting for education level, such as the analysis found in ''The Bell Curve'', fail to adjust for the quality of education. Not all high school graduates or college graduates have received the same quality of education. A 2006 study reported that years of education is an inadequate measure of the educational experience among multicultural elders, and that adjusting for quality of education greatly reduced the overall effect of racial differences on the tests.<ref>'''' Jennifer J. Manly, Diane M. Jacobs, ], Scott A. Small and Yaakov Stern</ref> A 2004 study reported that quality of education and cultural experience influence how older African Americans approach neuropsychological tasks and concluded that adjustment for these variables may improve specificity of neuropsychological measures.<ref>''Acculturation, Reading Level, and Neuropsychological Test Performance Among African American Elders'' Jennifer J. Manly, Desiree A. Byrd, Pegah Touradji, Yaakov Stern</ref> Yet another study reported that, although significant differences were observed between the ethnic groups when matched for years of education, equating for literacy level eliminated all performance differences between African Americans and European Americans on both cancellation tasks which assess visual scanning<ref>''Cancellation test performance in African American, Hispanic, and White elderly'' Desiree A. Byrd, Pegah Touradji, Ming-Xin Tang and Jennifer J. Manly</ref> (like reaction time tests, cancellation task tests are sometimes regarded as "culture free" tests of intelligence). Eric A. Hanushek and Steven G. Rivkin wrote in their 2006 book that unequal distributions of inexperienced teachers and of racial concentrations in schools can explain all of the increased achievement gap between grades 3 and 8.<ref>''School Quality and the Black-White Achievement Gap'' Eric A. Hanushek and Steven G. Rivkin 2006</ref> In recent years there have also been studies into the degrees in which many minorities, especially blacks, have internalized pathologies about their supposed lack of intelligence and the effects it has in their self-confidence, quality of learning and achievement.<ref></ref> Additionally, Jensen's studies (Jensen, 1974b) show that 7% of black children of black professionals have mean IQs below that of white children from low-income families, yet this seems to have little to no detriment on the black children's success.<ref></ref> | |||
=== Spearman's hypothesis === | |||
A 2004 study in ] found highly significant effects for both level and quality of education within the black African first language groups taking the ] IQ tests. The scores of black African first language groups with advantaged education were comparable with the US standardization, whereas scores for black African first language participants with disadvantaged education were significantly lower than this. The study cautioned that faulty conclusions may be drawn about the effects of ethnicity and the potential for ] ].<ref>''Cross-cultural Effects on IQ Test Performance: A Review and Preliminary Normative Indications on WAIS-III Test Performance'' Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology Volume 26, Number 7 / October 2004</ref> | |||
{{Main|Spearman's hypothesis}} | |||
Spearman's hypothesis states that the magnitude of the black–white difference in tests of cognitive ability depends entirely or mainly on the extent to which a test measures general mental ability, or ''g''. The hypothesis was first formalized by ], who devised the statistical "method of correlated vectors" to test it. If Spearman's hypothesis holds true, then the cognitive tasks that have the highest ''g''-load are the tasks in which the gap between black and white test takers are greatest. Jensen and Rushton took this to show that the cause of ''g'' and the cause of the gap are the same—in their view, genetic differences.{{sfn|Rushton|Jensen|2005}} | |||
{{harvtxt|Mackintosh|2011|pages=338–39}} acknowledges that Jensen and Rushton showed a modest correlation between ''g''-loading, heritability, and the test score gap, but does not agree that this demonstrates a genetic origin of the gap. Mackintosh argues that it is exactly those tests that Rushton and Jensen consider to have the highest ''g''-loading and heritability, such as the Wechsler test, that have seen the greatest increases in black performance due to the Flynn effect. This likely suggests that they are also the most sensitive to environmental changes, which undermines Jensen's argument that the black–white gap is most likely caused by genetic factors. {{harvtxt|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a|page=146}} make the same point, noting also that the increase in the IQ scores of black test takers necessarily indicates an increase in ''g''. | |||
==== Racial discrimination in education ==== | |||
Roslyn Arlin Mickelson writes that racial discrimination in education arises from actions of institutions or individual state actors, their attitudes and ideologies, or processes that systematically treat students from different racial/ethnic groups disparately or inequitably.<ref>''When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? A Social Science Perspective'' by Roslyn Arlin Mickelson University of North Carolina at Charlotte</ref> Despite advancement in education reform efforts, to this day African American students continue to experience inequities within the educational system. Hala Elhoweris, Kagendo Mutua, Negmeldin Alsheikh and Pauline Holloway conducted a study of the effect of students' ethnicity on teachers' educational decision making. The results of this study indicated that the student's ethnicity did make a difference in the teachers' referral decisions for ] educational programs.<ref>''Effect of Children's Ethnicity on Teachers' Referral and Recommendation Decisions in Gifted and Talented Programs'' Journal article by Negmeldin Alsheikh, Hala Elhoweris, Pauline Holloway, Kagendo Mutua; Remedial and Special Education, Vol. 26, 2005</ref> Recently, a number of scholars have examined the issue of disproportionate representation of minority students in ] programs<ref>(Salend, Garrick Duhaney, & Montgomery, 2002; Townsend, 2002)</ref><ref>''Racial Inequity in Special Education.'' Losen, Daniel J., Ed.; Orfield, Gary, Ed. Harvard Education Publishing Group.</ref> | |||
James Flynn argued that his findings undermine Spearman's hypothesis.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Flynn |first1=J.R. |year=1999 |title=Searching for justice: the discovery of IQ gains over time |url=http://www.stat.columbia.edu/~gelman/stuff_for_blog/flynn.pdf |url-status=live |journal=American Psychologist |volume=54 |pages=5–9 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.54.1.5 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20100625085640/http://www.stat.columbia.edu/~gelman/stuff_for_blog/flynn.pdf |archive-date=25 June 2010 |access-date=26 October 2017}}</ref> In a 2006 study, he and William Dickens found that between 1972 and 2002 "The standard measure of the ''g'' gap between Blacks and Whites declined virtually in tandem with the IQ gap."{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}} Flynn also criticized Jensen's basic assumption that a correlation between ''g''-loading and test score gap implies a genetic cause for the gap.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Flynn |first=James R. |year=2010 |title=The spectacles through which I see the race and IQ debate |url=http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/flynn2010a.pdf |journal=Intelligence |volume=38 |issue=4 |pages=363–366 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2010.05.001 |access-date=2011-02-18 |archive-date=2020-12-07 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20201207224050/http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/flynn2010a.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref> In a 2014 suite of meta-analyses, along with co-authors Jan te Nijenhuis and Daniel Metzen, he showed that the same negative correlation between IQ gains and ''g''-loading obtains for cognitive deficits of known environmental cause: ], ], ], and ].<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Flynn |first1=James R. |last2=te Nijenhuis |first2=Jan |last3=Metzen |first3=Daniel |date=2014 |title=The g beyond Spearman's g: Flynn's paradoxes resolved using four exploratory meta-analyses |url=https://james-flynn.net/wp-content/uploads/2020/12/flynn2014-The-g-beyond-Spearmans-g-Flynns-paradoxes-resolved-using-four-exploratory-meta-analyses.pdf |journal=Intelligence |volume=44 |pages=1–10|doi=10.1016/j.intell.2014.01.009 }}</ref> | |||
Teachers' perceptions of a students cultural background may affect school achievement. African American students with African American cultural backgrounds, for example, have been found to benefit from culturally responsive teaching.<ref>(Gay, 2000; Irvine & Armento, 2001; Ladson-Billings, 1994, 2001)</ref> In a 2003 study researchers found that teachers perceived students with African American culture-related movement styles as lower in achievement, higher in aggression, and more likely to need special education services than students with standard movement styles irrespective of race or other academic indicators.<ref>''The Effects of African American Movement Styles on Teachers' Perceptions and Reactions'' Journal article by Scott T. Bridgest, Audrey Davis Mccray, La Vonne I. Neal, Gwendolyn Webb-Johnson; Journal of Special Education, Vol. 37, 2003</ref> | |||
===Adoption studies=== | |||
A number of IQ studies have been done on the effect of similar rearing conditions on children from different races. The hypothesis is that this can be determined by investigating whether black children adopted into white families demonstrated gains in IQ test scores relative to black children reared in black families. Depending on whether their test scores are more similar to their biological or adoptive families, that could be interpreted as supporting either a genetic or an environmental hypothesis. Critiques of such studies question whether the environment of black children—even when raised in white families—is truly comparable to the environment of white children. Several reviews of the ] literature have suggested that it is probably impossible to avoid confounding biological and environmental factors in this type of study.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|p=337}} Another criticism by {{harvtxt|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a|pages=134}} is that adoption studies on the whole tend to be carried out in a restricted set of environments, mostly in the medium-high SES range, where heritability is higher than in the low-SES range. | |||
The ] (1976) examined the ] test scores of 122 ] children and 143 nonadopted children reared by advantaged white families. The children were restudied ten years later.<ref name="Weinberg 1992">{{harvnb|Weinberg|Scarr|Waldman|1992}}</ref>{{sfn|Scarr|Weinberg|1976}}{{sfn|Loehlin|2000|p=185}} The study found higher IQ for white people compared to black people, both at age 7 and age 17.<ref name="Weinberg 1992"/> Acknowledging the existence of confounding factors, Scarr and Weinberg, the authors of the original study, did not consider that it provided support for either the hereditarian or environmentalist view.{{sfn|Scarr|Weinberg|1990}} | |||
Ellis Cose writes that low expectations may have a negative impact on the achievement of minorities. He writes that black people did not need to read ''The Bell Curve'' to be aware of the low expectations held for them by the majority culture. He recalls examples of low expectations from his teachers in school who regarded his use of ] as "laziness" and teachers who did not feel it was important to purchase new text books because they did not expect the students to be able to read anything complex. He contrasts these low expectations with the high expectations philosophy of ] where, using the ideas Whimbey articulated in his book ''Intelligence can be Taught'' teachers created a program called SOAR. SOAR raised the performance of black students and lead Xavier to become the university that sends the greatest number of black students to medical school in the United States. The SOAR program produced gains equivalent to 120 points on an ]. Cose writes that "..we must treat people, whatever their color, as if they have unlimited intellectual capacity."<ref>''Color-Blind'' Ellis Cose. Page 50</ref> | |||
Three other studies lend support to environmental explanations of group IQ differences: | |||
] have been used to test for possible uncommon factors in the development of children belonging to different ethnic groups, which would include the results of racial discrimination. However, these tests have concluded that black, white, Hispanic and Asian children follow developmental processes which are "nearly identical."<ref>Rowe, David C., Vazsonyi, Alexander T. and Flannery, Daniel J. . Psychological Review 101,3 (July 1994): 396-413</ref><ref>Rowe, David C. and Hobart H. Cleveland. Intelligence Volume 23, Issue 3, November-December 1996, Pages 205-228.</ref> | |||
*{{harvp|Eyferth|1961}} studied the out-of-wedlock children of black and white soldiers stationed in Germany after World War II who were then raised by white German mothers in what has become known as the ]. He found no significant differences in average IQ between groups. | |||
*{{harvp|Tizard et al.|1972}} studied black (West Indian), white, and mixed-race children raised in British long-stay residential nurseries. Two out of three tests found no significant differences. One test found higher scores for non-white people. | |||
*{{harvp|Moore|1986}} compared black and mixed-race children adopted by either black or white middle-class families in the United States. Moore observed that 23 black and interracial children raised by white parents had a significantly higher mean score than 23 age-matched children raised by black parents (117 vs 104), and argued that differences in early socialization explained these differences. | |||
Frydman and Lynn (1989) showed a mean IQ of 119 for Korean infants adopted by Belgian families. After correcting for the ], the IQ of the adopted Korean children was still 10 points higher than that of the Belgian children.{{sfn|Loehlin|2000|p=187}}<ref name="Frydman and Lynn">{{cite journal |author=Frydman and Lynn |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |volume=10 |issue=12 |pages=1323–1325 |year=1989 |title=The intelligence of Korean children adopted in Belgium |doi=10.1016/0191-8869(89)90246-8}}</ref> | |||
==== Caste-like minorities ==== | |||
The book '']'' claims that it is not lower average intelligence that leads to the lower status of ethnic minorities, it is instead their lower status that leads to their lower average intelligence test scores. The following table from the same book compares social status or ] position to test scores and school success in countries around the world.<ref name="bell myth">'''' by Claude S. Fischer, Michael Hout, Martín Sánchez Jankowski, Samuel R. Lucas, Ann Swidler, and Kim Vos. Page 192. (The footnotes given are also from this book.)</ref> | |||
Reviewing the evidence from adoption studies, Mackintosh finds that environmental and genetic variables remain confounded and considers evidence from adoption studies inconclusive, and fully compatible with a 100% environmental explanation.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|page=337}} Similarly, Drew Thomas argues that race differences in IQ that appear in adoption studies are in fact an artifact of methodology, and that East Asian IQ advantages and black IQ disadvantages disappear when this is controlled for.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Thomas |first1=Drew |year=2017 |title=Racial IQ Differences among Transracial Adoptees: Fact or Artifact? |journal=Journal of Intelligence |volume=5 |issue=1 |page=1 |doi=10.3390/jintelligence5010001 |pmid=31162392 |pmc=6526420 |doi-access=free}}</ref> | |||
{| border="0" cellpadding="3" cellspacing="0" align="center" width="60%" | |||
|+'''Group Differences Around the World''' | |||
|- | |||
! | | |||
! colspan="2" style="background:#FFD147" | Status or Caste Position | |||
! colspan="2" style="background:#00CC99" | Test Scores, School Success | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCCCFF" | '''Country''' | |||
| style="background:#FFFFCC" align="center" | '''High''' | |||
| style="background:#FFFFCC" align="center" | '''Low''' | |||
| style="background:#CCFFE6" align="center" | '''High''' | |||
| style="background:#CCFFE6" align="center" | '''Low''' | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | ]<ref>''The Bell Curve'' and many other places.</ref> | |||
| ] | |||
| ] | |||
| Whites | |||
| Blacks | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | | |||
| Asians | |||
| ] | |||
| Asians | |||
| Latinos | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | | |||
| ] | |||
| ] | |||
| East Asians | |||
| Southeast Asians | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | | |||
| Whites | |||
| ]<ref>Church ''Academic Achievement''</ref> | |||
| Whites | |||
| American Indians | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | ]<ref>Lynn et al. ''Home Background''</ref> | |||
| ]s | |||
| ]s | |||
| Protestants | |||
| Catholics | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | ]<ref>Klich ''Aboriginal Cognition and Psychological Science''; Clark and Halford, ''Does Cognitive Style Account for Cultural Differences?''</ref> | |||
| Whites | |||
| ] | |||
| Whites | |||
| Aborigines | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | ]<ref>Ogbu, ''Minority Education and Caste''</ref> | |||
| Whites | |||
| ] | |||
| Whites | |||
| Maori | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | ]<ref>Verster and Prinsloo, ''The Diminishing Test Performance Gap''</ref> | |||
| English | |||
| ] | |||
| English | |||
| ] | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | ]<ref>Raven, ''The Raven Progressive Matrices'' esp fig. 2</ref> | |||
| ] | |||
| ] | |||
| Flemish | |||
| French | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | ]<ref>Kugelmass et al., ''Patterns of Intellectual Ability''</ref> | |||
| ] | |||
| ] | |||
| Jews | |||
| Arabs | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | | |||
| Western Jews | |||
| Eastern Jews | |||
| Western Jews | |||
| Eastern Jews | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | ]<ref>Das and Khurana, ''Catse and Cognitive Processes''</ref> | |||
| Nontribals | |||
| Tribal people | |||
| Nontribals | |||
| Tribal people | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | | |||
| ] | |||
| ] | |||
| Brahmin | |||
| Dalit | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | | |||
| High caste | |||
| Low caste | |||
| High caste | |||
| Low caste | |||
|- | |||
| style="background:#CCE6FF" align="right" | ]<ref>Adamovic, ''Intellectual Development and Level of Knowledge in Gypsy Pupils''</ref> | |||
| ] | |||
| ] | |||
| Slovaks | |||
| Gypsies | |||
|- | |||
|colspan="5" | | |||
<small> | |||
Notes: | |||
#Czechoslovakia split into the ] and ] in 1993. | |||
#The Flemish are not a minority in Belgium. The ] 2006 results for schoolchildren 15 years of age showed that the Flemish outperformed the ] (French).<ref>http://www.pisa.oecd.org/dataoecd/31/60/39705905.xls</ref> | |||
# Afrikaans speakers make up the majority of the white population; most South Africans, however, identified themselves in the census as black, and speak one of the ] as their ]<ref>http://www.statssa.gov.za/census01/html/RSAPrimary.pdf; South African Census Results, see graph on pg. 8 (15 in the.pdf)</ref> | |||
</small> | |||
|} | |||
=== |
===Racial admixture studies=== | ||
Most people have ancestry from different geographical regions. In particular, African Americans typically have ancestors from both Africa and Europe, with, on average, 20% of their genome inherited from European ancestors.<ref>{{harvnb|Bryc et al.|2009}}</ref> If racial IQ gaps have a partially genetic basis, one might expect black people with a higher degree of European ancestry to score higher on IQ tests than black people with less European ancestry, because the genes inherited from European ancestors would likely include some genes with a positive effect on IQ.{{sfn|Loehlin|2000}} Geneticist ] has argued that an experiment based on the Mendelian "common garden" design, where specimens with different hybrid compositions are subjected to the same environmental influences, are the only way to definitively show a causal relation between genes and group differences in IQ. Summarizing the findings of admixture studies, he concludes that they have shown no significant correlation between any cognitive ability and the degree of African or European ancestry.{{sfn|Templeton|2001}} | |||
{{Cleanup-section|date=April 2008}} | |||
Given the observed differences in IQ scores between certain groups, a great deal of debate revolves around the significance of these observations. Various interpretations of test data lead to a multitude of conflicting conclusions as to which specific explanations the data support. | |||
Studies have employed different ways of measuring or approximating relative degrees of ancestry from Africa and Europe. Some studies have used skin color as a measure, and others have used blood groups. {{harvp|Loehlin|2000}} surveys the literature and argues that the blood groups studies may be seen as providing some support to the genetic hypothesis, even though the correlation between ancestry and IQ was quite low. He finds that studies by {{harvp|Eyferth|1961}}, Willerman, Naylor & Myrianthopoulos (1970) did not find a correlation between degree of African/European ancestry and IQ. The latter study did find a difference based on the race of the mother, with children of white mothers with black fathers scoring higher than children of black mothers and white fathers. Loehlin considers that such a finding is compatible with either a genetic or an environmental cause. All in all Loehlin finds admixture studies inconclusive and recommends more research. | |||
The range of views among contemporary scholars include: | |||
* ] argues that differences in cognitive abilities between races exist and are of social/environmental origin. | |||
* ] argues that differences in cognitive abilities between races exist and are caused by both social/environmental disparities and also genetic differences between races. | |||
* ] and ], amongst others, argue that race is not a biological concept, but rather is socially constructed, and hence measurements of IQ differences between races cannot reflect an underlying ] biological cause.<ref>Lieberman, Leonard; Alice Littlefield and Larry T. Reynolds. "The Debate over Race: Thirty Years and Two Centuries Later." in "Race and I.Q." (1975) Ashley Montague (ed.) ISBN 0-19-510220-7. ""Those who study I.Q. scores of different groups do so with the assumption that there are homogeneous races, when in fact that is not the case. Populations being very similar to each other to begin with, and being interbred with eah other - and humans have interbred throughout their evolution - makes it impossible to explain differences in I.Q. scores largely on the basis of heredity".</ref><ref name="montagu">] "The I.Q. Mythology" in "Race and I.Q." (1975) Ashley Montagu (ed) ISBN 0-19-510220-7: "I have stated, and I think the statement long overdue, that both the term "race" and the term "IQ" are delusive because in the one case the social conception of "race" was the deliberate invention of a slave-owning caste attempting to justify its conduct, and in the other case because "IQ" tests do not measure what they have generally been claimed to measure, namely, innate intelligence ... The statistical treatment of data in any investigation may be quite unexceptional, but when unexceptional statistical methods are applied to the analysis of unsound data to begin with, based on assumptions that are equally unsound, one can only end up with conclusions that are equally unsound. Such are the erroneous constructs of "race" and "IQ"."</ref><ref name="jlgraves">] "The Race Myth: Why we Pretend Race Exists in America". (2004) ], ]. p174. "''The Bell Curve's'' arguments are plausible only if one assumes that biologically defined races exist within our species and that they correspond to the socially defined American races, that IQ really does determine the majority of the differential in social stature in societies, and that IQ tests reliably measure all pertinent aspects of cognitive function and are unbiased.</ref> | |||
* ] argues that the use of differences in test scores to argue for race differences in intelligence is a case of the inappropriate use of tests in different groups. | |||
Reviewing the evidence from admixture studies {{harvp|Hunt|2010}} considers it to be inconclusive because of too many uncontrolled variables. {{harvtxt|Mackintosh|2011|p=338}} quotes a statement by {{harvp|Nisbett|2009}} to the effect that admixture studies have not provided a shred of evidence in favor of a genetic basis for the IQ gap. | |||
Researchers who believe that there is no significant genetic contribution to race differences in intelligence include: | |||
:Flynn (1980), Brody (1992), Neisser et al. (1996), Nisbett (1998), Mackintosh (1998), Jencks and Phillips (1998), and Fish (2002). | |||
===Mental chronometry=== | |||
Scientists who emphasize cultural explanations do not necessarily exclude a small genetic influence. Reynolds (2000) suggests up to 20% genetic influence be included in the cultural explanation. | |||
{{Main|Mental chronometry}} | |||
] measures the elapsed time between the presentation of a sensory stimulus and the subsequent behavioral response by the participant. These studies have shown inconsistent results when comparing black and white populations groups, with some studies showing whites outperforming blacks, and others showing blacks outperforming whites.{{sfn|Sheppard|Vernon|2008}} | |||
Arthur Jensen argued that this reaction time (RT) is a measure of the speed and efficiency with which the brain processes information,<ref name="Jensen 2006">{{harvnb|Jensen|2006}}</ref> and that scores on most types of RT tasks tend to correlate with scores on standard IQ tests as well as with ''g''.<ref name="Jensen 2006" /> Nisbett argues that some studies have found correlations closer to 0.2, and that a correlation is not always found.<ref name="Nisbett 2009">{{harvnb|Nisbett|2009}}</ref> Nisbett points to the {{harvp|Jensen|Whang|1993}} study in which a group of Chinese Americans had longer reaction times than a group of European Americans, despite having higher IQs. Nisbett also mentions findings in {{harvp|Flynn|1991}} and {{harvp|Deary|2001}} suggesting that movement time (the measure of how long it takes a person to move a finger after making the decision to do so) correlates with IQ just as strongly as reaction time, and that average movement time is faster for black people than for white people.{{sfn|Nisbett|2009|pp=221–2}} {{harvtxt|Mackintosh|2011|page=339}} considers reaction time evidence unconvincing and comments that other cognitive tests that also correlate well with IQ show no disparity at all, for example the habituation/] test. He further comments that studies show that rhesus monkeys have shorter reaction times than American college students, suggesting that different reaction times may not tell us anything useful about intelligence. | |||
Researchers who believe that there are significant genetic contributions to race differences in intelligence include: | |||
:McGurk (1953), Garrett (1961), Shuey (1966), Shockley (1968), Eysenck (1971), Baker (1974), Loehlin et al. (1975), Vernon (1979), Lynn (1991a), Waldman et al. (1994), Scarr (1995), Levin (1997), Jensen (1998b), Rushton (2000), and Gottfredson (2005b).<ref>http://en.wikipedia.org/Talk:Race_and_intelligence/references</ref> | |||
===Brain size=== | |||
Coming advances in ] and ] are expected to soon provide the ability to test hypotheses about group differences more rigorously than has as yet been possible.<ref>Pinker (2006), Rowe (2005), Stock (2002) pp. 44–47.</ref> | |||
{{main|Brain size}} | |||
A number of studies have reported a moderate statistical correlation between differences in IQ and brain size between individuals in the same group.{{sfn|Deary|Penke|Johnson|2010}}{{sfn|McDaniel|2005}} Some scholars have reported differences in average brain sizes between racial groups,{{sfn|Ho et al.|1980}} although this is unlikely to be a good measure of IQ as brain size also differs between men and women, but without significant differences in IQ.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}} At the same time newborn black children have the same average brain size as white children, suggesting that the difference in average size could be accounted for by differences in environment.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}} Several environmental factors that reduce brain size have been demonstrated to disproportionately affect black children.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}} | |||
===Archaeological data=== | |||
]'s '']'' instead argues that historical differences in economic and technological development for different geographic areas can be explained by differences in geography (which affects factors like population density and spread of new technology) and differences in available crops and domesticatable animals. ] argues in his 2004 ''The Geography of Thought'' that some of these regional differences shaped lasting cultural traits, such as the collectivism required by East Asian rice ], compared with the individualism of ] herding, maritime mercantilism, and money crops wine and olive oil<ref>(pp. 34-35).</ref> However, it has been suggested that these environmental differences may operate in part by ] for higher levels of IQ.<ref>This theory is discussed by Jensen (1998b) (pp. 435-437), Lynn (1991b) and Rushton (2000) in general and by both Wade (2006) and with respect to ''Guns, Germs, and Steel''. See ]. .. Voight et al. (2006) state generally that "a number of recent studies have detected more signals of adaptation in non-African populations than in Africans, and some of those studies have conjectured that non-Africans might have experienced greater pressures to adapt to new environments than Africans have" (Kayser et al. 2003, Akey et al. 2004, Storz et al. 2004, Stajich and Hahn 2005, Carlson et al. 2005).</ref> | |||
Archaeological evidence does not support claims by Rushton and others that black people's cognitive ability was inferior to white people's during prehistoric times.{{sfn|MacEachern|2006}} | |||
==Policy relevance and ethics== | |||
], psychology professor at the ] and head of the ], has written a controversial book called ]: A Life History Perspective. Rushton claims in the book that race is a valid biological concept and that racial differences frequently arrange in a continuum of ] (Orientals, East Asians) at one extreme, ] (blacks, Africans) at the opposite extreme, and ] (whites, Europeans) in the middle.<ref name=reb2ndabridged>{{cite book | |||
{{Main|Intelligence and public policy}} | |||
| author = Rushton, J. P. | |||
The ] of research on race and intelligence has long been a subject of debate: in a 1996 report of the ];{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} in guidelines proposed by Gray and Thompson and by Hunt and Carlson;<ref name="Hunt & Carlson 2007"/><ref>{{harvnb|Gray|Thompson|2004}}</ref> and in two editorials in ] in 2009 by ] and by ] and ].<ref name="Ceci & Williams 2009">{{harvnb|Ceci|Williams|2009}}</ref><ref name="Rose 2009">{{Cite journal |last=Rose |first=Steven |date=2009 |title=Should scientists study race and IQ? NO: Science and society do not benefit |url=https://rdcu.be/dj5uC |journal=Nature |volume=457 |issue=7231 |pages=786–788 |doi=10.1038/457786a |pmid=19212384 |bibcode=2009Natur.457..786R |s2cid=42846614 |url-access=limited}}</ref> | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| coauthors = | |||
| year = 1995 | |||
| month = | |||
| title = Race, Evolution, and Behavior: A Life History Perspective | |||
| chapter = | |||
| editor = | |||
| others = | |||
| edition = 2nd special abridged | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher = Charles Darwin Research Institute | |||
| location = Port Huron, MI | |||
| id = | |||
| url = http://www.charlesdarwinresearch.org/Race_Evolution_Behavior.pdf |format=PDF}}</ref> It has been ]. | |||
] maintains that the history of ] makes this field of research difficult to reconcile with current ethical standards for science.<ref name="Rose 2009"/> | |||
Differing rates of economic growth have also been attributed to numerous factors other than racial IQ gaps such as local availability of resources, climate, and sociopolitical factors. See for example the ], the ], and the ] or works by ],<ref>Pomeranz, Kenneth (2001). ''The Great Divergence''. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.</ref> ],<ref>Jones, Eric (1997). ''The European Miracle''. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref> ],<ref>Mokyr, Joel (1992). ''The Lever of Riches''. Oxford: Oxford University Press.</ref> and ].<ref>North, Douglass (1976). ''The Rise of the Western World''. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.</ref> | |||
On the other hand, ] has argued that had there been a ban on research on possibly poorly conceived ideas, much valuable research on intelligence testing (including his own discovery of the ]) would not have occurred.<ref>{{harvnb|Flynn|2009b}}</ref> | |||
Many have argued for increased interventions in order to close the gaps.<ref name="Brookings">{{cite web |last1=Jencks |first1=Christopher |last2=Phillips |first2=Meredith |title=The Black-White Test Score Gap |url=https://www.nytimes.com/books/first/j/jencks-gap.html |website=New York Times |access-date=2 October 2016 |archive-date=8 October 2016 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20161008015238/https://www.nytimes.com/books/first/j/jencks-gap.html |url-status=live }}</ref> Flynn writes that "America will have to address all the aspects of black experience that are disadvantageous, beginning with the regeneration of inner city neighborhoods and their schools."<ref name="Flynn 2008">{{harvnb|Flynn|2008}}</ref> Especially in developing nations, society has been urged to take on the prevention of cognitive impairment in children as a high priority. Possible preventable causes include ], ] such as ], ], cerebral ], ] ] and ], newborn ], ], head injuries, ] and ].<ref name="Olness 2003">{{harvnb|Olness|2003}}</ref> | |||
== Criticisms == | |||
=== Outdated methodology === | |||
A 2006 paper by Professor Denny Borsboom argues that mainstream contemporary test analysis does not reflect substantial recent developments in the field and "bears an uncanny resemblance to the psychometric state of the art as it existed in the 1950s."<ref>. Denny Borsboom. Psychometrika Vol. 71, No. 3, 425–440. September 2006.</ref> It also claims that some of the most influential recent studies on group differences in intelligence, in order to show that the tests are unbiased, use outdated methodology. In particular the reliance on ] rather than more sophisticated measurement models as found in ]. In response to criticism proponents of the genetic hypothesis claim they use a standard for intelligence known as g. g is measured by performance on test items without the influence of language or math. | |||
=== Utility of research === | |||
Theories of race and intelligence have been challenged on grounds of their ]. Critics want to know what purpose such research could serve and why it has been an intense an area of focus for a few researchers. Some defend the research, saying it has egalitarian aims. IQ is, after all, a predictor of educational achievement and special needs. Accordingly, as a matter of public policy, resources can be better allocated by reducing the data to better understand the challenges; it is considered a ]. Still others say that the true motivation for the research is the same as that of the ] movement and other forms of ].<ref name="autogenerated6">e.g., Sternberg, 2003, pp. 386–387</ref><ref name="autogenerated6" /> Even supporters of intelligence research have described such research as analogous to "working with dynamite" or "dangerous play" in sports.<ref>Hunt & Carlson, in press</ref> | |||
Jensen and Rushton have justified their research in this area as being necessary to answer the question of how much white racism should be held responsible for ethnic groups’ unequal performance in certain areas. They maintain that when racism is blamed for disparities which are the result of biological differences, the result is mutual resentment, and unjustified punishment of the more successful group. They state: <blockquote>he view that one segment of the population is largely to blame for the problems of another segment can be even more harmful to racial harmony, by first producing demands for compensation and thereby inviting a backlash. Equating group disparities in success with racism on the part of the more successful group guarantees mutual resentment. As overt discrimination fades, still large racial disparities in success lead Blacks to conclude that White racism is not only pervasive but also insidious because it is so unobservable and “unconscious.” Whites resent that nonfalsifiable accusation and the demands to compensate Blacks for harm they do not believe they caused.<ref name="30yrs240"/></blockquote> | |||
Regarding whether research in this area is desirable, ] wrote in 1992, "Research on racial differences in intelligence is desirable ''if'' the research is appropriately motivated, honestly done, and adequately communicated." Defenders of the research suggest that both scientific curiosity and a desire to draw benefits from the research are appropriate motivations.{{citation needed|date=March 2008}} Researchers such as ] have suggested that conclusions from the research can help make political decisions, such as the type of educational opportunities and expectations of achievement policy makers should have for people of different races.{{citation needed|date=March 2008}} ], a political scientist of the ] has used their conclusions to criticize social programs based on racial equality that fail, he claims, to recognize the realities of racial differences.{{citation needed|date=March 2008}} | |||
=== Test construction === | |||
While the existence of average IQ test score differences has been a matter of accepted fact for decades, a great deal of controversy exists among scholars over the question of whether these score differences reflected real differences in cognitive ability. Some claim that there is no evidence for test bias since IQ tests are equally good predictors of IQ-related factors (such as school performance) for U.S. Blacks and Whites.<ref name="APA">Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J. Jr., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J. et al. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist, 51, 77–101.</ref> The performance differences persist in tests and testing situations in which care has been taken to eliminate bias.<ref name="APA"/> It has also been suggested that IQ tests are formulated in such a way as to disadvantage minorities.<ref name="APA" /> Controlled studies have shown that test construction does not substantially contribute to the IQ gap.<ref name="APA" /> However, some psychometricians are not satisfied that the question of test bias is fully answered by these results.<ref>Dolan, C. V. (1997). A note on Schönemann's refutation of Spearman's hypothesis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32, 319–325.</ref><ref>Dolan, C. V. (2000). Investigating Spearman's hypothesis by means of multi-group confirmatory factor analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 35, 21–50.</ref><ref>Dolan, C. V., & Hamaker, E. L. (2001). Investigating Black-White differences in psychometric IQ: Multi-group confirmatory factor analyses of WISC-R and K-ABC and a critique of the method of correlated vectors. In F. Columbus (Ed.), Advances in psychology research (Vol. 6, pp. 30–59). Huntington, NY: Nova Science.</ref> | |||
The preponderance of evidence indicates that IQ tests measuring ] are crossculturally valid. There is little or no evidence of population-specific cultural effects apart from the obvious example of language bias.<ref>http://www.charlesdarwinresearch.org/PRSL2007.pdf</ref> For example, ] et al. found that the IQ of 12- to 15-year-old Kenyans predicted school grades at about the same level as they do in the West.<ref>Sternberg, R. J., Nokes, C., Geissler, P. W., Prince, R., | |||
Okatcha, F., Bundy, D. A. & Grigorenko, E. L. 2001 The | |||
relationship between academic and practical intelligence: | |||
a case study in Kenya. Intelligence 29, 401–418.</ref> IQ also predicted university performance equally well in African and non-African engineering students in South Africa in a 2004 study.<ref>Construct validity of Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices for African and non-African engineering students in South Africa.</ref> Salgado et al. (2003) demonstrated the international generalizability of general mental ability across 10 member countries of the ] and differences in a nation’s culture, religion, language, socioeconomic level or employment legislation did not affect the predictive validity of IQ tests.<ref>Salgado, J. F., Anderson, N., Moscoso, S., Bertua, C. & | |||
Fruyt, F. D. 2003 International validity generalization of | |||
GMA and cognitive abilities: a European community | |||
meta-analysis. Pers. Psychol. 56, 573–605.</ref> | |||
However, other studies have found evidence for bias. A 2005 study finds some evidence that the WAIS-R is not culture-fair for Mexican Americans.<ref> Steven P. Verney Assessment, Vol. 12, No. 3, 303-319 (2005)</ref> Other recent studies have questioned the culture-fairness of IQ tests when used in South Africa.<ref> Shuttleworth-Edwards AB, Kemp RD, Rust AL, Muirhead JG, Hartman NP, Radloff SE. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2004 Oct;26(7):903-20.</ref><ref> 1*, Leah K. Hamilton1, Betty R. Onyura1 and Andrew S. Winston International Journal of Selection and Assessment Volume 14 Issue 3 Page 278 - September 2006</ref> | |||
=== Source of funding === | |||
Proponents of genetic explanations of race/IQ correlation, such as Rushton, Lynn, and Jensen, have often been criticized for receiving funding from the ], a group that had ties to Nazis and the eugenics of the early 20th century. The ] considers the ] to be a ]. Rushton is the current head of the Pioneer Fund and has spoken at conferences of the ], in which he has also published articles.<ref>http://www.splcenter.org/intel/intelreport/article.jsp?pid=214#27</ref> Anti-racist '']'' described one of these articles as a "veritable 'who’s who' of American ].".<ref>http://www.searchlightmagazine.com/index.php?link=template&story=162</ref> | |||
Proponents of genetic explanations of race-IQ correlation have in turn accused their critics of ] in the name of ]. They claim harassment and interference with both their work and funding. The Pioneer Fund, whose stated purpose is "to advance the scientific study of heredity and human differences", makes "no grants to individuals but only to research institutions, mainly universities, mostly for specialized 'niche' projects, which have difficulty attracting funds from government sources or from larger foundations".<ref></ref> | |||
==See also== | ==See also== | ||
*] | * ] | ||
* ] | |||
*] | |||
*] | * ] | ||
* ] | |||
*] | |||
* ] | |||
*'']'' | |||
*'']'' | |||
*'']'' | |||
== Notes == | |||
<!--See http://en.wikipedia.org/Wikipedia:Footnotes for an explanation of how to generate footnotes using the <ref(erences/)> tags--> | |||
{{reflist|2}} | |||
== References == | == References == | ||
{{refbegin|2}} | |||
=== Notes === | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
{{notelist}} | |||
| author = Akey, J. M., Eberle, M. A., Rieder, M. J., Carlson, C. S., Shriver, M. D., Nickerson, D. A. and Kruglyak, L. | |||
| year = 2004 | |||
| title = Population History and Natural Selection Shape Patterns of Genetic Variation in 132 Genes | |||
| journal = PLoS Biology | |||
| volume = 2 | |||
| issue = 10 | |||
| pages = e286 | |||
| doi = 10.1371/journal.pbio.0020286 | |||
| pmid = 15361935 | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Bean, R. B. | |||
| year = 1906 | |||
| title = Some Racial Peculiarities of the Negro Brain | |||
| journal = American Journal of Anatomy | |||
| volume = 5 | |||
| pages = 353–432 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = | doi = 10.1002/aja.1000050402 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Berry, J. W. | |||
| year = 1966 | |||
| title = Temne and Eskimo Perceptual Skills | |||
| journal = International Journal of Psychology | |||
| volume = 1 | |||
| pages = 207–222 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = | doi = 10.1080/00207596608247156 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = ] | |||
| year = 1873 | |||
| title = Sur Les Crânes De La Caverne De ’Homme Mort (Loere) | |||
| journal = Revue d’Anthropologie | |||
| volume = 2 | |||
| pages = 1–53 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Brody, N. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| year = 1992 | |||
| title = Intelligence | |||
| edition = | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher = Academic | |||
| location = San Diego, CA | |||
| url = }} | |||
* Carlson CS, Thomas DJ, Eberle MA, Swanson JE, Livingston RJ, ''et al.'' (2005). . ''Genome Res'' 15: 1553–1565. PMID 16251465 | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Carretta, T. R. and Ree, M. J. | |||
| year = 1995 | |||
| month = August | |||
| title = Near Identity of Cognitive Structure in Sex and Ethnic Groups | |||
| journal = Personality and Individual Differences | |||
| volume = 19 | |||
| issue = 2 | |||
| pages = 149–155 | |||
| doi = 10.1016/0191-8869(95)00031-Z | |||
| url = | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Chan, J. and Lynn, R. | |||
| year = 1989 | |||
| month = | |||
| title = The Intelligence of Six-Year-Olds in Hong Kong | |||
| journal = Journal of Biosocial Science | |||
| volume = 21 | |||
| issue = | |||
| pages = 461–464 | |||
| pmid = 2808473 | |||
| url = | |||
| doi = 10.1017/S0021932000018198 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Cochran, G., Hardy, J., and Harpending, H. | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| title = Natural History of Ashkenazi Intelligence | |||
| journal = Journal of Biosocial Science | |||
| volume = 38 | |||
| issue = 5 | |||
| pages = 659 | |||
| doi = 10.1017/S0021932005027069 | |||
| url = http://homepage.mac.com/harpend/.Public/AshkenaziIQ.jbiosocsci.pdf | |||
|format=PDF}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Colom, R., Lluis-Font, J.M., and Andrés-Pueyo, A. | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| title = The generational intelligence gains are caused by decreasing variance in the lower half of the distribution: Supporting evidence for the nutrition hypothesis | |||
| journal = Intelligence | |||
| volume = 33 | |||
| pages = 83–91 | |||
| doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.010 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Degler, C. N. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| year = 1992 | |||
| title = In Search of Human Nature:The Decline and Revival of Darwinism in American Social Thought | |||
| edition = | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher = Oxford University Press | |||
| location = New York | |||
| isbn = 0-415-28659-X | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Dickens, W. T. | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| month = Spr | |||
| title = Genetic Differences and School Readiness | |||
| journal = Future of Children | |||
| volume = 15 | |||
| issue = 1 | |||
| pages = 55–69 | |||
| pmid = 2005-04825-005 | |||
| url = http://muse.jhu.edu/demo/future_of_children/v015/15.1dickens.pdf |format=PDF}} | |||
* {{cite journal|author=Dickens, W.T., and J.R. Flynn | year=In press | title=Black Americans reduce the racial IQ gap: Evidence from standardized samples | journal=Psychological Science | url=http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_IQ.pdf|format=PDF}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Dolan, C. V. | |||
| year = 1997 | |||
| title = A Note on Schönemann's Refutation of Spearman's Hypothesis | |||
| journal = Multivariate Behavioral Research | |||
| volume = 32 | |||
| pages = 319–325 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = | |||
| doi = 10.1207/s15327906mbr3203_4 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Dolan, C. V. | |||
| year = 2000 | |||
| title = Investigating Spearman's Hypothesis by Means of Multi-Group Confirmatory Factor Analysis | |||
| journal = Multivariate Behavioral Research | |||
| volume = 35 | |||
| pages = 21–50 | |||
| doi = 10.1207/S15327906MBR3501_2 | |||
| url = | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Dolan, C. V. and Hamaker, E. L. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| coauthors = | |||
| year = 2001 | |||
| title = Advances in Psychological Research | |||
| chapter = Investigating Black-White Differences in Psychometric IQ: Multi-Group Confirmatory Factor Analyses of the WISC-R and K-Abc, and a Critique of the Method of Correlated Vectors | |||
| editor = F. Columbus | |||
| edition = | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher = ], Inc. | |||
| location = Huntington, NY | |||
| isbn = 1-56072-897-3 | |||
| url = http://users.fmg.uva.nl/cdolan/wch.pdf |format=PDF}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Espanshade, T. J., Chung, C. Y. | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| month = June | |||
| title = The Opportunity Cost of Admission Preferences at Elite Universities | |||
| journal = Social Science Quarterly | |||
| volume = 86 | |||
| issue = 2 | |||
| pages = 293–305 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = http://opr.princeton.edu/faculty/tje/espenshadessqptii.pdf | |||
|format=PDF| doi = 10.1111/j.0038-4941.2005.00303.x | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Flynn, J. R. | |||
| authorlink = James R. Flynn | |||
| coauthors = | |||
| year = 1980 | |||
| title = Race, IQ and Jensen | |||
| publisher = Routledge & Kegan Paul | |||
| location = London | |||
| isbn = 0-7100-0651-9 | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Flynn, J. R. | |||
| year = 1987 | |||
| title = Massive IQ Gains in 14 Nations: What IQ Tests Really Measure | |||
| journal = Psychological Bulletin | |||
| volume = 101 | |||
| pages = 171–191 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = | |||
| doi = 10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.171 | |||
}} | |||
* Flynn, J. R. (1987b). Race and IQ: Jensen's case refuted. In S. Modgil & C. Modgil (Eds.), Arthur Jensen: Consensus and controversy (pp. 221–232). Lewes, England: Falmer Press. | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Flynn, J. R. | |||
| year = 1999 | |||
| title = Searching for Justice: The Discovery of IQ Gains over Time | |||
| journal = American Psychologist | |||
| volume = 54 | |||
| pages = 5–20 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = | |||
| doi = 10.1037/0003-066X.54.1.5 | |||
}} | |||
* Flynn, J. R. (1999b). Reply to Rushton: A gang of gs overpowers factor analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 391–393. | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Gottfredson, L. S. | |||
| year = 1997 | |||
| month = Jan-February | |||
| title = Mainstream Science on Intelligence: An Editorial with 52 Signatories, History, and Bibliography (Reprinted from the Wall Street Journal, 1994) | |||
| journal = Intelligence | |||
| volume = 24 | |||
| issue = 1 | |||
| pages = 13–23 | |||
| doi = 10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90011-8 | |||
| url = http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/1997mainstream.pdf |format=PDF}} appeared originally as in ''Wall Street Journal'', December 13, 1994. | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Gottfredson, L. S. | |||
| year = 2005b | |||
| title = What If the Hereditarian Hypothesis Is True | |||
| journal = Psychology, Public Policy, and Law | |||
| volume = 11 | |||
| pages = 311–319 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/2005hereditarian-hypothesis.pdf | |||
|format=PDF| doi = 10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.311 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Hacker, Andrew | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| month = November 3 | |||
| title = The Truth About the Colleges | |||
| journal = The New York Review of Books | |||
| volume = 52 | |||
| issue = 17 | |||
| url = http://www.nybooks.com/articles/18401 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Herrnstein, R. J. and Murray, C. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| coauthors = | |||
| year = 1994 | |||
| title = The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life | |||
| edition = | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher = Free Press | |||
| location = New York | |||
| isbn = 0-02-914673-9 | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Jank, Wolfgang | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| coauthors = Bruce L. Golden, Paul F. Zantek | |||
| year = 2004 | |||
| month = December | |||
| title = Old World vs. New World: Evolution of Nobel Prize Shares | |||
| publisher = Department of Decision & Information Technologies, University of Maryland | |||
| url = http://www.smith.umd.edu/faculty/wjank/NobelShares.pdf|format=PDF}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Jensen, A. R. | |||
| authorlink = Arthur Jensen | |||
| year = 1969 | |||
| title = How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement | |||
| journal = Harvard Educational Review | |||
| volume = 39 | |||
| pages = 1–123 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Jensen, A. R. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| year = 1973 | |||
| title = Educability and Group Differences | |||
| publisher = Methuen | |||
| location = London | |||
| isbn = 0-06-012194-7 | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Jensen, A. R. | |||
| year = 1993 | |||
| month = January–March | |||
| title = Spearman's Hypothesis Tested with Chronometric Information-Processing Tasks | |||
| journal = Intelligence | |||
| volume = 17 | |||
| issue = 1 | |||
| pages = 47–77 | |||
| doi = 10.1016/0160-2896(93)90039-8 | |||
| url = | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | author = Jensen, A. R. | |||
| authorlink = Arthur Jensen | |||
| title = The ''g'' Factor: The Science of Mental Ability | |||
| url = | |||
| year = 1998b | |||
| publisher = Praeger Publishers | |||
| location = Westport, CT | |||
| isbn = 0-275-96103-6 | |||
| pages = }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Jensen, A. R. and Whang, P. A. | |||
| year = 1994 | |||
| title = Speed of Accessing Arithmetic Facts in Long-Term Memory: A Comparison of Chinese-American and Anglo-American Children | |||
| journal = Contemporary Educational Psychology | |||
| volume = 19 | |||
| pages = 1–12 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = | |||
| doi = 10.1006/ceps.1994.1001 | |||
}} | |||
* jinfo.org (2004). "," Accessed 9 June 2005. | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Jones, Garett, and Schneider, Joel W | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| month = June | |||
| title = Intelligence, Human Capital, and Economic Growth: a Bayesian Averaging of Classical Estimates (BACE) Approach | |||
| url = http://www.siue.edu/~garjone/JonesSchneApr.pdf| format = {{Dead link|date=June 2008}} – <sup></sup> }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Kayser, M., Brauer, S. and Stoneking, M. | |||
| year = 2003 | |||
| month = June | |||
| title = A genome scan to detect candidate regions influenced by local natural selection in human populations | |||
| journal = Mol Biol Evol | |||
| volume = 20 | |||
| issue = 6 | |||
| pages = 893–900 | |||
| doi = 10.1093/molbev/msg092 | |||
| pmid = 12717000 | |||
| url = http://mbe.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/full/20/6/893 }} | |||
* ] ''How Free is Free?'' Harvard University Press: 2009 ISBN 9780674031524 | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = ], Lindzey, G. and Spuhler, J. N. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| coauthors = | |||
| year = 1975 | |||
| title = Race Differences in Intelligence | |||
| edition = | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher = W. H. Freeman | |||
| location = San Francisco | |||
| isbn = 0-7167-0754-3 | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Lynn, R. | |||
| year = 1991a | |||
| title = Race Differences in Intelligence: A Global Perspective | |||
| journal = Mankind Quarterly | |||
| volume = 31 | |||
| pages = 255–296 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = http://www.mugu.com/cgi-bin/Upstream/People/Lynn/lynn-race-iq.html | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Lynn, R. | |||
| year = 1991b | |||
| month = | |||
| title = The Evolution of Racial Differences in Intelligence | |||
| journal = Mankind Quarterly | |||
| volume = 32 | |||
| issue = | |||
| pages = 99–173 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = http://66.102.7.104/search?q=cache:sq.4mg.com/LynnIQdiff.htm | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Lynn, R. | |||
| authorlink = Richard Lynn | |||
| year = 2001 | |||
| title = The Science of Human Diversity: A History of the Pioneer Fund | |||
| edition = | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher = University Press of America | |||
| location = Lanham, MD | |||
| isbn = 0-7618-2041-8 | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite book | author = Lynn, R. | year = 2006 | month = In press| title = Race Differences in Intelligence: An Evolutionary Analysis| chapter = | editor = | others = | edition = | pages = | publisher = Washington Summit Books| location = | id = | url = }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Lynn, R., Hampson, S., and Magee, M.| year = 1984 | |||
| month = | |||
| title = Home Background, Intelligence, Personality and Education as Predictors of Unemployment in Young People | |||
| journal = Personality and Individual Differences | |||
| volume = 5 | |||
| issue = | |||
| pages = 549–57 | |||
| doi = 10.1016/0191-8869(84)90029-1 | |||
| url = | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Lynn, R. and Holmshaw, M. | |||
| year = 1990 | |||
| month = | |||
| title = Black-White Differences in Reaction Times and Intelligence | |||
| journal = Social Behavior and Personality | |||
| volume = 18 | |||
| issue = | |||
| pages = 299–308 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = | |||
| doi = 10.2224/sbp.1990.18.2.299 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Lynn, R. and Shigehasa, T. | |||
| year = 1991 | |||
| month = | |||
| title = Reaction Times and Intelligence: A Comparison of Japanese and British Children | |||
| journal = Journal of Biosocial Science | |||
| volume = 23 | |||
| issue = | |||
| pages = 409–416 | |||
| pmid = 1939289 | |||
| url = | |||
| doi = 10.1017/S0021932000019519 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Lynn, R. and Vanhanen, T. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| coauthors = | |||
| year = 2002 | |||
| title = IQ and the Wealth of Nations | |||
| edition = | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher = Praeger | |||
| location = Westport, CT | |||
| isbn = 0-275-97510-X | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = MacArthur, R. S. | |||
| year = 1968 | |||
| title = Some Differential Abilities of Northern Canadian Native Youth | |||
| journal = International Journal of Psychology | |||
| volume = 3 | |||
| pages = 43–51 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = | |||
| doi = 10.1080/00207596808246642 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Mackintosh, N. J. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| year = 1998 | |||
| title = IQ and human intelligence | |||
| edition = | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher = Oxford University Press | |||
| location = Oxford; New York | |||
| isbn = 0-19-852368-8 | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Mall, F. P. | |||
| year = 1909 | |||
| title = On Several Anatomical Characters of the Human Brain, Said to Vary According to Race and Sex, with Special Reference to the Weight of the Frontal Lobe | |||
| journal = American Journal of Anatomy | |||
| volume = 9 | |||
| pages = 1–32 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = | |||
| doi = 10.1002/aja.1000090102 | |||
}} | |||
* "Innately Inferior or Equal?" in ''The Struggle for Equality'' by ] historian ] | |||
(Princeton University Press: 1964) | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Morton, S. G. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| year = 1839 | |||
| title = Crania Americana; or, a Comparative View of the Skulls of Various Aboriginal Nations of North and South America. To Which Is Prefixed an Essay on the Varieties of the Human Species | |||
| edition = | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher = Simpkin Marshall & Co. | |||
| location = Philadelphia | |||
| id = | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Murray, C. | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| month = September | |||
| title = The Inequality Taboo | |||
| journal = Commentary Magazine | |||
| volume = 120 | |||
| issue = 2 | |||
| pages = 13–22 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = http://www.commentarymagazine.com/production/files/murray0905.html | |||
| format = {{Dead link|date=June 2008}} – <sup></sup> | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Murray, C. and Herrnstein, R. J. | |||
| year = 1994 | |||
| month = October 31 | |||
| title = Race, Genes and IQ - an Apologia | |||
| journal = New Republic | |||
| volume = 211 | |||
| issue = 18 | |||
| pages = 27–37 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = http://www.mugu.com/cgi-bin/Upstream/atoms/xtra1/herrnstein-murray-tnr.html | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Neisser, U. | |||
| year = 1997 | |||
| title = Never a Dull Moment | |||
| journal = American Psychologist | |||
| volume = 52 | |||
| pages = 79–81 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = http://ist-socrates.berkeley.edu/~maccoun/PP279_Neisser2.html | doi = 10.1037/0003-066X.52.1.79 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. J., Boykin, A. W., Brody, N., Ceci, S. J., Halpern, D. F., Loehlin, J. C., Perloff, R., Sternberg, R. J. and Urbina, S. | |||
| year = 1996 | |||
| month = February | |||
| title = Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns | |||
| journal = American Psychologist | |||
| volume = 51 | |||
| issue = 2 | |||
| pages = 77–101 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = http://www.psych.uiuc.edu/~broberts/Neisser%20et%20al,%201996,%20intelligence.pdf |format=PDF| doi = 10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Nisbett, R. E. | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| title = Heredity, Environment, and Race Differences in IQ: A Commentary on Rushton and Jensen (2005) | |||
| journal = Psychology, Public Policy, and Law | |||
| volume = 11 | |||
| pages = 302–310 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = http://taxa.epi.umn.edu/~mbmiller/journals/pppl/200504/2/302-2.html | |||
| doi = 10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.302 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Ogbu, J. U. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| year = 1978 | |||
| title = Minority Education and Caste: The American System in Cross-Cultural Perspective | |||
| publisher = Academic Press | |||
| location = San Diego, CA | |||
| id = | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Pearl, R. | |||
| year = 1934 | |||
| title = The Weight of the Negro Brain | |||
| journal = Science | |||
| volume = 80 | |||
| pages = 431–434 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = | |||
| doi = 10.1126/science.80.2080.431 | |||
| pmid = 17816046 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Pinker, Steven | |||
| year = 2006 | |||
| month = January | |||
| title = The Edge Annual Question — 2006: What is Your Dangerous Idea? | |||
| journal = The Edge | |||
| url = http://www.edge.org/q2006/q06_3.html#pinker }} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E. and McGuffin, P. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| coauthors = | |||
| year = 2001 | |||
| title = Behavioral Genetics (4th Ed.) | |||
| edition = | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher = Freeman | |||
| location = New York | |||
| isbn = 0-7167-5159-3 | |||
| url = }} | |||
* Purdey, J. E., ] (2002). http://eprints.unimelb.edu.au/archive/00000840/ | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Reynolds, C. R., Chastain, R. L., Kaufman, A. S., & McLean, J. E. | |||
| year = 1987 | |||
| title =Demographic characteristics and IQ among adults: Analysis of the WAIS-R standardization sample as a function of the stratification variables | |||
| journal = Journal of School Psychology | |||
| volume = 25 | |||
| pages = 323–342 | |||
| doi =10.1016/0022-4405(87)90035-5 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Roth, P. L., Bevier, C. A., Bobko, P., Switzer, F. S. and Tyler, P. | |||
| year = 2001 | |||
| month = Sum | |||
| title = Ethnic Group Differences in Cognitive Ability in Employment and Educational Settings: A Meta-Analysis | |||
| journal = Personnel Psychology | |||
| volume = 54 | |||
| issue = 2 | |||
| pages = 297–330 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = | |||
| doi = 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2001.tb00094.x | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Rowe, D. C. | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| month = January | |||
| title = Under the Skin: On the Impartial Treatment of Genetic and Environmental Hypotheses of Racial Differences | |||
| journal = American Psychologist | |||
| volume = 60 | |||
| issue = 1 | |||
| pages = 60–70 | |||
| pmid = 15641922 | |||
| url = | |||
| doi = 10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.60 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Rushton, J. P. | |||
| year = 1989a | |||
| title = Japanese Inbreeding Depression Scores: Predictors of Cognitive Differences between Blacks and Whites | |||
| journal = Intelligence | |||
| volume = 13 | |||
| pages = 43–51 | |||
| doi = 10.1016/0160-2896(89)90005-6 | |||
| url = | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Rushton, J. P. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| year = 2000 | |||
| title = Race, Evolution, and Behavior: A Life History Perspective | |||
| edition = 3rd | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher = Charles Darwin Research Institute | |||
| location = Port Huron, MI | |||
| isbn = 0-9656836-1-3 | |||
| url = http://www.charlesdarwinresearch.org/ }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Rushton, J. P. | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| title = (Book review) Lynn Richard, Race Differences in Intelligence: An Evolutionary Analysis | |||
| journal = Personality and Individual Differences | |||
| volume = | |||
| issue = | |||
| pages = In press | |||
| id = | |||
| url = http://www.ssc.uwo.ca/psychology/faculty/rushtonpdfs/In%20Press%20Review%20of%20Lynn%20in%20PAID.pdf | |||
|format=PDF}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Rushton, J. P. and Jensen, A. R. | |||
| year = 2003 | |||
| title = African-White IQ Differences from Zimbabwe on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised Are Mainly on the g Factor | |||
| journal = Personality and Individual Differences | |||
| volume = 34 | |||
| pages = 177–183 | |||
| doi = 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00192-7 | |||
| url = http://www.ssc.uwo.ca/psychology/faculty/rushtonpdfs/Rushton-Jensen2003PAID.pdf | |||
|format=PDF}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Rushton, J. P. and Jensen, A. R. | |||
| year = 2005a | |||
| month = June | |||
| title = Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability | |||
| journal = Psychology, Public Policy, and Law | |||
| volume = 11 | |||
| issue = | |||
| pages = 235–294 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = http://taxa.epi.umn.edu/~mbmiller/journals/pppl/200504/2/235-2.html | |||
| doi = 10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Smedley, A. and Smedley, B. D. | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| month = January | |||
| title = Race as Biology Is Fiction, Racism as a Social Problem Is Real: Anthropological and Historical Perspectives on the Social Construction of Race | |||
| journal = American Psychologist | |||
| volume = 60 | |||
| issue = 1 | |||
| pages = 16–26 | |||
| doi = 10.1037/0003-066x.60.1.16 | |||
| url = http://www.apa.org/journals/releases/amp60116.pdf | |||
|format=PDF}} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Snyderman, M. and Rothman, S. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| coauthors = | |||
| year = 1988 | |||
| title = The IQ Controversy, the Media, and Public Policy | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher = Transaction Books | |||
| location = New Brunswick, NJ | |||
| isbn = 0-88738-839-6 | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Sowell, T. | |||
| authorlink = | |||
| coauthors = | |||
| year = 1981 | |||
| title = Ethnic America: A History | |||
| edition = | |||
| pages = | |||
| publisher =Basic Books | |||
| location =New York | |||
| isbn = 0-465-02075-5 | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Stajich, J. E. and Hahn, M. W. | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| month = January | |||
| title = Disentangling the effects of demography and selection in human history | |||
| journal = Molecular Biology and Evolution | |||
| volume = 22 | |||
| issue = 1 | |||
| pages = 63–73 | |||
| pmid = 15356276 | |||
| doi = 10.1093/molbev/msh252 }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Sternberg, R. J. | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| month = | |||
| title = There Are No Public-Policy Implications: A Reply to Rushton and Jensen (2005) | |||
| journal = Psychology, Public Policy, and Law | |||
| volume = 11 | |||
| issue = | |||
| pages = e72 | |||
| id = | |||
| url = http://taxa.epi.umn.edu/~mbmiller/journals/pppl/200504/2/295-2.html | |||
| doi = 10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.295 | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Stock, Gregory | |||
| authorlink = Gregory Stock | |||
| year = 2002 | |||
| title = Redesigning Humans: Our Inevitable Genetic Future | |||
| edition = | |||
| pages = 194 | |||
| publisher =Houghton Mifflin | |||
| location =Boston | |||
| isbn = 0-618-06026-X | |||
| url = }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Storz, J. F., Payseur, B. A. and Nachman, M. W. | |||
| year = 2004 | |||
| title = Genome Scans of DNA Variability in Humans Reveal Evidence for Selective Sweeps Outside of Africa | |||
| journal = Molecular Biology and Evolution | |||
| volume = 21 | |||
| issue = 9 | |||
| pages = 1800–1811 | |||
| doi = 10.1093/molbev/msh192 | |||
| url = http://mbe.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/abstract/21/9/1800 | |||
| pmid = 15201398 }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Suzuki, L. and Aronson, J. | |||
| year = 2005 | |||
| month = June | |||
| title = The Cultural Malleability of Intelligence and Its Impact on the Racial/Ethnic Hierarchy | |||
| journal = Psychology, Public Policy, and Law | |||
| volume = 11 | |||
| issue = 2 | |||
| pages = 320–327 | |||
| doi = 10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.320 | |||
| url = http://taxa.epi.umn.edu/~mbmiller/journals/pppl/200504/2/320-2.html }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Vint, F. W. | |||
| year = 1934 | |||
| title = The Brain of the Kenya Native | |||
| journal = Journal of Anatomy | |||
| volume = 48 | |||
| pages = 216–223 | |||
| url = | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Voight, B. F., Kudaravalli, S., Wen, X., Pritchard, J. K. | |||
| year = 2006 | |||
| month = March | |||
| title = A Map of Recent Positive Selection in the Human Genome | |||
| journal = PLoS Biology | |||
| volume = 4 | |||
| issue = 3 | |||
| pages = e72 | |||
| doi = 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040072 | |||
| url = | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | author = Wade, Nicholas | year = 2006 | title = Before the Dawn: Recovering the Lost History of Our Ancestors | publisher = Penguin Press |location = New York | isbn = 1-59420-079-3 }} | |||
* {{cite journal | |||
| author = Wicherts, J. M., Dolan, C. V., Hessen, D. J., Oosterveld, P., van Baal, G. C. M., Boomsma, D. I. and Span, M. M. | |||
| year = 2004 | |||
| title = Are intelligence tests measurement invariant over time? Investigating the nature of the Flynn effect | |||
| journal = Intelligence | |||
| volume = 32 | |||
| issue = 5 | |||
| pages = 509–537 | |||
| doi = 10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.002 | |||
| url = | |||
}} | |||
* {{cite book | |||
| author = Wright, W. D. | |||
| year = 2007 | |||
| title = Crisis of the Black Intellectual | |||
| journal = | |||
| volume = | |||
| issue = | |||
| pages = ISBN 0–88378–251–0 | |||
| doi = | |||
| pmid = | |||
| url = http://www.wdwrightbooks.com | |||
| nopp = true }} | |||
=== Citations === | |||
{{Reflist}} | |||
=== Bibliography === | |||
{{Refbegin|30em|indent=yes}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Alland |first=Alexander Jr |url=https://archive.org/details/raceinmindraceiq00alla |title=Race in Mind |publisher=Palgrave Macmillan |year=2002 |pages= |isbn=978-0-312-23838-4 |url-access=registration }} | |||
*{{Cite web |url=http://www.aaanet.org/stmts/race.htm |title=American Anthropological Association Statement on 'Race' and Intelligence |date=December 1994 |publisher=American Anthropological Association |ref={{harvid|AAA|1994}} |access-date=2005-03-24 |archive-date=2005-03-05 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20050305114339/http://www.aaanet.org/stmts/race.htm |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite web |url=http://www.aaanet.org/stmts/racepp.htm |title=American Anthropological Association Statement on 'Race' |date=17 May 1998 |publisher=American Anthropological Association |ref={{harvid|AAA|1998}} |access-date=24 February 2011 |archive-date=27 June 2013 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130627184228/http://www.aaanet.org/stmts/racepp.htm |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |year=1996 |title=AAPA Statement on Biological Aspects of Race |url=http://www.physanth.org/association/position-statements/biological-aspects-of-race/?searchterm=race |journal=American Journal of Physical Anthropology |volume=101 |issue=4 |pages=569–570 |doi=10.1002/ajpa.1331010408 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110928030510/http://www.physanth.org/association/position-statements/biological-aspects-of-race/?searchterm=race |archive-date=2011-09-28 |access-date=2011-05-24 |ref={{harvid|AAPA|1996}} }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last1=Aronson |first1=E |title=Social Psychology |last2=Wilson |first2=TD |last3=Akert |first3=AM |publisher=Prentice Hall |year=2005 |isbn=978-0-13-178686-8 |edition=5th |location=Upper Saddle River, NJ}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Behrman |first1=JR |last2=Alderman |first2=H |last3=Hoddinott |first3=J |year=2004 |title=Hunger and Malnutrition |url=http://www.copenhagenconsensus.com/Files/Filer/CC/Papers/sammendrag/Accepted__Hunger_summary_070504.pdf |journal=Copenhagen Consensus |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20061025122932/http://www.copenhagenconsensus.com/Files/Filer/CC/Papers/sammendrag/Accepted__Hunger_summary_070504.pdf |archive-date=2006-10-25 }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Bellinger |first1=David C |last2=Stiles |first2=Karen M |last3=Needleman |first3=Herbert L |date=December 1992 |title=Low-Level Lead Exposure, Intelligence and Academic Achievement: A Long-term Follow-up Study |url=http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/cgi/content/abstract/90/6/855 |journal=Pediatrics |volume=90 |issue=6 |pages=855–61 |doi=10.1542/peds.90.6.855 |pmid=1437425 |s2cid=25746146 |author-link1=David Bellinger |author-link3=Herbert Needleman |access-date=2009-12-19 |archive-date=2019-01-04 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190104021959/http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/90/6/855?sso=1&sso_redirect_count=1&nfstatus=401&nftoken=00000000-0000-0000-0000-000000000000&nfstatusdescription=ERROR%3A+No+local+token |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Berlet |first=Chip |date=Summer 2003 |title=Into the Mainstream |url=http://www.splcenter.org/intel/intelreport/article.jsp?aid=50 |journal=Intelligence Report |issue=110 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20100202152214/http://www.splcenter.org/intel/intelreport/article.jsp?aid=50 |archive-date=2 February 2010 |access-date=21 February 2012 |df=dmy-all |author-link=Chip Berlet }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Block |first=Ned |title=Race and Intelligence: Separating Science from Myth |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates |year=2002 |editor-last=Fish |editor-first=Jefferson |chapter=How heritability misleads about race}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Borsboom |first=Denny |date=September 2006 |title=The attack of the psychometricians |journal=Psychometrika |volume=71 |issue=3 |pages=425–40 |doi=10.1007/s11336-006-1447-6 |pmc=2779444 |pmid=19946599}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Brace |first=C Loring |year=1999 |title=An Anthropological Perspective on 'Race' and Intelligence: The non-clinal nature of human cognitive capabilities |journal=Journal of Anthropological Research |volume=55 |issue=2 |pages=245–64 |doi=10.1086/jar.55.2.3631210 |jstor=3631210 |s2cid=83112950 |author-link=C. Loring Brace}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Brace |first=C. Loring |title=Race is a four letter word |publisher=Oxford University Press |year=2005 |isbn=978-0-19-517351-2 |page=326}} | |||
*{{Cite encyclopedia |year=2012 |title=race |encyclopedia=Encyclopædia Britannica |publisher=Encyclopædia Britannica Inc |url=http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/488030/race |edition=Online |ref={{harvid|Britannica|2012}} |access-date=2011-02-24 |archive-date=2011-06-29 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110629075541/http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/488030/race |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Brown |first1=Robert T |last2=Reynolds |first2=Cecil R |last3=Whitaker |first3=Jean S |year=1999 |title=Bias in Mental Testing since "Bias in Mental Testing" |journal=School Psychology Quarterly |volume=14 |issue=3 |pages=208–38 |doi=10.1037/h0089007|s2cid=46561407 }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Bryc |first1=Katarzyna |last2=Auton |first2=Adam |last3=Nelson |first3=Matthew R. |last4=Oksenberg |first4=Jorge R. |last5=Hauser |first5=Stephen L. |last6=Williams |first6=Scott |last7=Froment |first7=Alain |last8=Bodo |first8=Jean-Marie |last9=Wambebe |first9=Charles |last10=Tishkoff |first10=Sarah A. |last11=Bustamante |first11=Carlos D. |year=2009 |title=Genome-wide patterns of population structure and admixture in West Africans and African Americans |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America |volume=107 |issue=2 |pages=786–91 |bibcode=2010PNAS..107..786B |doi=10.1073/pnas.0909559107 |pmc=2818934 |pmid=20080753 |ref={{harvid|Bryc et al.|2009}} |doi-access=free}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Campbell |first1=FA |last2=Ramey |first2=CT |year=1994 |title=Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic achievement: A follow-up study of children from low-income families |journal=Child Development |volume=65 |issue=2 |pages=684–698 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00777.x}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Campbell |first1=Frances A |last2=Ramey |first2=Craig T |last3=Pungello |first3=Elizabeth |last4=Sparling |first4=Joseph |last5=Miller-Johnson |first5=Shari |year=2002 |title=Early Childhood Education: Young Adult Outcomes From the Abecedarian Project |journal=Applied Developmental Science |volume=6 |pages=42–57 |doi=10.1207/s1532480xads0601_05 |s2cid=71602425 |ref={{harvid|Campbell et al.|2002}}}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last1=Carson |first1=Michael |title='Race', IQ and Genes |last2=Beckwith |first2=Jon |date=2001 |publisher=John Wiley & Sons, Ltd |isbn=978-0-470-01590-2 |pages=1–5 |language=en |doi=10.1002/9780470015902.a0005689.pub3}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Ceci |first1=SJ |last2=Williams |first2=WM |year=2009 |title=Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? Yes: the scientific truth must be pursued |journal=Nature |volume=457 |issue=7231 |pages=788–9 |bibcode=2009Natur.457..788C |doi=10.1038/457788a |pmid=19212385 |s2cid=205044224 |author-link1=Stephen J. Ceci |author-link2=Wendy M. Williams |doi-access=free}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Cooper |first=R. S. |year=2005 |title=Race and IQ: Molecular Genetics as Deus ex Machina |journal=American Psychologist |volume=60 |issue=1 |pages=71–76 |citeseerx=10.1.1.624.5059 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.71 |pmid=15641923}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Cottrell |first1=Jonathan M. |last2=Newman |first2=Daniel A. |last3=Roisman |first3=Glenn I. |date=November 2015 |title=Explaining the black-white gap in cognitive test scores: Toward a theory of adverse impact |journal=The Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=100 |issue=6 |pages=1713–1736 |doi=10.1037/apl0000020 |issn=1939-1854 |pmid=25867168}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Cronshaw |first1=Steven F |last2=Hamilton |first2=Leah K |last3=Onyura |first3=Betty R |last4=Winston |first4=Andrew S |date=September 2006 |title=Case for Non-Biased Intelligence Testing Against Black Africans Has Not Been Made: A Comment on Rushton, Skuy, and Bons |journal=International Journal of Selection and Assessment |volume=14 |issue=3 |pages=278–287 |doi=10.1111/j.1468-2389.2006.00346.x |s2cid=91179275 |ref={{harvid|Cronshaw et al.|2006}}}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last1=Daley |first1=C. E. |title=The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence |last2=Onwuegbuzie |first2=A. J. |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=2011 |isbn=978-0-521-51806-2 |editor-last=Sternberg |editor-first=R. |location=Cambridge New York |pages=293–306 |chapter=Race and Intelligence |editor-last2=Kaufman |editor-first2=S. B.}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Deary |first=Ian J |title=Intelligence: A Very Short Introduction |publisher=Oxford University Press |year=2001 |isbn=978-0-19-289321-5}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Deary |first1=I. J. |last2=Penke |first2=L. |last3=Johnson |first3=W. |year=2010 |title=The neuroscience of human intelligence differences |url=https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/8895819/the_neuroscience_of_human_intelligence.pdf |journal=Nature Reviews Neuroscience |volume=11 |issue=3 |pages=201–211 |doi=10.1038/nrn2793 |pmid=20145623 |s2cid=5136934 |hdl=20.500.11820/9b11fac3-47d0-424c-9d1c-fe6f9ff2ecac |hdl-access=free |access-date=2019-01-26 |archive-date=2018-07-19 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180719195613/https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/8895819/the_neuroscience_of_human_intelligence.pdf |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Deary |first1=IJ |last2=Johnson |first2=W |last3=Houlihan |first3=LM |year=2009 |title=Genetic foundations of human intelligence |url=https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/8895353/genetic_foundations_of_human_intelligence.pdf |journal=Human Genetics |volume=126 |issue=1 |pages=215–32 |doi=10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4 |pmid=19294424 |hdl=20.500.11820/c3e0a75b-dad6-4860-91c6-b242221af681 |s2cid=4975607 |hdl-access=free |access-date=2019-12-07 |archive-date=2020-08-01 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200801215610/https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/8895353/genetic_foundations_of_human_intelligence.pdf |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Dickens |first1=William T |last2=Flynn |first2=James R |year=2001 |title=Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved |journal=Psychological Review |volume=108 |issue=2 |pages=346–69 |citeseerx=10.1.1.139.2436 |doi=10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346 |pmid=11381833 |author-link1=William T. Dickens}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Dickens |first1=William T |last2=Flynn |first2=James R |year=2006 |title=Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence from Standardization Samples |url=http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_iq.pdf |journal=Psychological Science |volume=17 |issue=10 |pages=913–20 |citeseerx=10.1.1.186.2540 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01802.x |pmid=17100793 |s2cid=6593169 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20091009095003/http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_IQ.pdf |archive-date=2009-10-09 }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Elhoweris |first1=Hala |last2=Mutua |first2=Kagendo |last3=Alsheikh |first3=Negmeldin |last4=Holloway |first4=Pauline |year=2005 |title=Effect of Children's Ethnicity on Teachers' Referral and Recommendation Decisions in Gifted and Talented Programs |journal=Remedial and Special Education |volume=26 |pages=25–31 |doi=10.1177/07419325050260010401 |s2cid=146321193 |ref={{harvid|Elhoweris et al.|2005}}}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Eppig |first=Christopher |year=2011 |title=Why Is Average IQ Higher in Some Places? |url=http://www.scientificamerican.com/article.cfm?id=why-is-average-iq-higher-in-some-places |journal=Scientific American |access-date=2011-11-20 |archive-date=2020-04-01 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200401001619/http://www.scientificamerican.com/article/why-is-average-iq-higher-in-some-places/ |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Evans |first=Gavin |title=Skin Deep: Journeys in the Divisive Science of Race |date=2019 |isbn=978-1-78607-622-9 |location=London |oclc=1059232398 |publisher=]}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Eyferth |first=K |year=1961 |title=Leistungern verscheidener Gruppen von Besatzungskindern Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest für Kinder (HAWIK) |journal=Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie |language=de |volume=113 |pages=222–41}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Eysenck |first=Hans J |title=Arthur Jensen: Consensus and controversy |publisher=Falmer |year=1987 |editor-last=Modgil |editor-first=S |location=New York, NY |chapter=Intelligence and Reaction Time: The Contribution of Arthur Jensen |editor-last2=Modgil |editor-first2=C |author-link=Hans Eysenck}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Falk |first=Avner |title=Anti-semitism: a history and psychoanalysis of contemporary hatred |publisher=Praeger |year=2008 |isbn=978-0-313-35385-7 |page=312}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Flynn |first=James R |year=1991 |title=Reaction times show that both Chinese and British children are more intelligent than one another |journal=Perceptual and Motor Skills |volume=72 |issue=2 |pages=544–6 |doi=10.2466/pms.1991.72.2.544 |s2cid=144091744 |author-link=James Flynn (academic)}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Flynn |first=James R |date=3 September 2008 |title=Perspectives: Still a question of black vs white? |url=http://www.cambridgeblog.org/2008/09/james-flynn-in-the-new-scientist/ |journal=New Scientist |edition=magazine issue |issue=2672 |doi=10.1016/S0262-4079(08)62253-8 |volume=199 |pages=48–50 |access-date=6 February 2011 |archive-date=7 July 2011 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110707111349/http://www.cambridgeblog.org/2008/09/james-flynn-in-the-new-scientist/ |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Flynn |first=James R |year=2009b |title=Would you wish the research undone? |journal=Nature |volume=458 |issue=7235 |page=146 |bibcode=2009Natur.458..146F |doi=10.1038/458146a |pmid=19279612 |doi-access=free}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Flynn |first=James R |year=2010 |title=The spectacles through which I see the race and IQ debate |url=http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/flynn2010a.pdf |journal=Intelligence |volume=38 |issue=4 |pages=363–6 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2010.05.001 |access-date=2011-02-18 |archive-date=2020-12-07 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20201207224050/http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/flynn2010a.pdf |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Flynn |first=James R. |title=Are We Getting Smarter? Rising IQ in the Twenty-First Century |publisher=] |year=2012 |isbn=978-1-107-60917-4 |location=Cambridge |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/arewegettingsmar0000flyn }} | |||
**{{lay source |template=cite periodical |author=Lea Winerman |date=March 2013 |title=Smarter than ever? |volume=44 |issue=3 |url=http://www.apa.org/monitor/2013/03/smarter.aspx |periodical=Monitor on Psychology}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Gottfredson |first=Linda S |year=1997 |title=Mainstream Science on Intelligence (editorial) |url=http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/1997mainstream.pdf |journal=Intelligence |volume=24 |pages=13–23 |doi=10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90011-8 |author-link=Linda Gottfredson |access-date=2005-01-08 |archive-date=2014-12-22 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20141222052006/http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/1997mainstream.pdf |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Gottfredson |first=Linda S |year=2007 |title=Applying Double Standards to 'Divisive' Ideas: Commentary on Hunt and Carlson |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |volume=2 |issue=2 |pages=216–220 |doi=10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00039.x |pmid=26151962 |s2cid=21203607}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Gould |first=Stephen Jay |url=https://archive.org/details/mismeasureofman00goulrich |title=The Mismeasure of Man |publisher=Norton |year=1981 |isbn=978-0-393-30056-7 |location=New York, London |url-access=registration |author-link=Stephen Jay Gould }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Graves |first=Joseph L. |url=https://archive.org/details/emperorsnewcloth00grav |title=The Emperor's New Clothes: Biological Theories of Race at the Millennium |publisher=Rutgers University Press |year=2001 |isbn=978-0-8135-2847-2 |edition=Kindle |url-access=registration |author-link=Joseph L. Graves Jr. }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Graves |first=Joseph L |title=Race and Intelligence: Separating Myth from Reality |publisher=Laurence Erlbaum Associates |year=2002a |isbn=978-0-8058-3757-5 |editor-last=Fish |editor-first=Jefferson M |pages=57–94 |chapter=The Misuse of Life History Theory: JP Rushton and the Pseudoscience of Racial Hierarchy}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Graves |first=Joseph L Jr |year=2002b |title=What a tangled web he weaves: Race, reproductive strategies and Rushton's life history theory |journal=Anthropological Theory |volume=2 |issue=2 |pages=131–54 |doi=10.1177/1469962002002002627 |s2cid=144377864}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Gray |first1=Jeremy R |last2=Thompson |first2=Paul M |year=2004 |title=Neurobiology of intelligence: science and ethics |url=https://www.mun.ca/biology/scarr/Gray_%26_Thompson_2004_Nature_Rev_Neurosci_5%2C471-482.pdf |journal=Nature Reviews Neuroscience |volume=5 |issue=6 |pages=471–82 |doi=10.1038/nrn1405 |pmid=15152197 |s2cid=2430677 |access-date=2019-05-10 |archive-date=2018-07-14 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180714065640/http://www.mun.ca/biology/scarr/Gray_%26_Thompson_2004_Nature_Rev_Neurosci_5%2C471-482.pdf |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last1=Grossman |first1=James B |title=The evolution of intelligence |last2=Kaufman |publisher=Routledge |year=2001 |isbn=978-0-8058-3267-9 |editor-last=Sternberg |editor-first=Robert J |chapter=Evolutionary Psychology: Promise and Perils |editor-last2=Kaufman |editor-first2=James C}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Hampshire |first1=A. |last2=Highfield |first2=R. R. |last3=Parkin |first3=B. L. |last4=Owen |first4=A. M. |year=2012 |title=Fractionating human intelligence |journal=Neuron |volume=76 |pages=1225–1237 |doi=10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.022 |pmid=23259956 |number=6 |doi-access=free}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last1=Herrnstein |first1=Richard J |url=https://archive.org/details/bellcurveintelli00herr |title=The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life |last2=Murray |first2=Charles |publisher=Free Press |year=1994 |isbn=978-0-02-914673-6 |location=New York |url-access=registration |author-link1=Richard Herrnstein |author-link2=Charles Murray (political scientist) }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Ho |first1=KC |last2=Roessmann |first2=U |last3=Straumfjord |first3=JV |last4=Monroe |first4=G |date=December 1980 |title=Analysis of brain weight. I. Adult brain weight in relation to sex, race, and age. |journal=Archives of Pathology & Laboratory Medicine |volume=104 |issue=12 |pages=635–9 |pmid=6893659 |ref={{harvid|Ho et al.|1980}}}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Hunt |first=Earl |title=Human Intelligence |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=2010 |isbn=978-0-521-70781-7 |author-link=Earl B. Hunt}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Hunt |first1=Earl |last2=Carlson |first2=Jerry |year=2007 |title=Considerations relating to the study of group differences in intelligence |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |volume=2 |issue=2 |pages=194–213 |doi=10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00037.x |pmid=26151960 |s2cid=32540018}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Hunt |first1=Earl |last2=Wittmann |first2=Werner |date=January–February 2008 |title=National intelligence and national prosperity |journal=Intelligence |volume=36 |issue=1 |pages=1–9 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.11.002}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Irvine |first=SH |year=1983 |title=Where intelligence tests fail |journal=Nature |volume=302 |issue=5907 |page=371 |bibcode=1983Natur.302..371I |doi=10.1038/302371b0 |s2cid=41604729 |doi-access=free}} | |||
*{{Cite book |url=https://archive.org/details/humanabilitiesin0000unse |title=Human Abilities in Cultural Context |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=1988 |isbn=978-0-521-34482-1 |editor-last=Irvine |editor-first=SH |editor-last2=Berry |editor-first2=JW }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Ivanovic |first1=Daniza M. |last2=Leiva |first2=Boris P. |last3=Pérez |first3=Hernán T. |last4=Olivares |first4=Manuel G. |last5=Dı́az |first5=Nora S. |last6=Urruti |first6=Marı́a Soledad C. |last7=Almagià |first7=Atilio F. |last8=Toro |first8=Triana D. |last9=Miller |first9=Patricio T. |last10=Bosch |first10=Enrique O. |last11=Larraı́n |first11=Cristián G. |year=2004 |title=Head size and intelligence, learning, nutritional status and brain development. Head, IQ, learning, nutrition and brain |journal=Neuropsychologia |volume=42 |issue=8 |pages=1118–31 |doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.022 |pmid=15093150 |s2cid=2114185 |ref={{harvid|Ivanovic et al.|2004}}}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last1=Jackson |first1=John P. |title=Race, Racism, and Science: Social Impact and Interaction |last2=Weidman |first2=Nadine M. |publisher=ABC-CLIO |year=2004 |page=23}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Jackson |first=John P. |title=Science for Segregation: Race, Law, and the Case against Brown v. Board of Education |publisher=] |year=2005 |isbn=978-0-8147-4271-6}} | |||
**{{lay source |template=cite web |title=Book Review: Science for Segregation: Race, Law, and the Case Against Brown v. Board of Education |url=http://www.historycooperative.org/journals/lhr/25.2/br_19.html |website=History Cooperative}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Jensen |first=Arthur R |year=1969 |title=How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement? |journal=Harvard Educational Review |volume=39 |pages=1–123 |doi=10.17763/haer.39.1.l3u15956627424k7 |title-link=How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement? |author-link=Arthur Jensen}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Jensen |first=Arthur R |title=Educability and Group Differences |publisher=Methuen |year=1973 |isbn=978-0-06-012194-5 |location=London |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/educabilitygroup0000jens }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Jensen |first1=Arthur R |last2=Whang |first2=PA |year=1993 |title=Reaction times and intelligence: a comparison of Chinese-American and Anglo-American children |journal=Journal of Biosocial Science |volume=25 |issue=3 |pages=397–410 |doi=10.1017/s0021932000020721 |pmid=8360233|s2cid=18171436 }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Jensen |first=Arthur R |url=https://archive.org/details/gfactorscienc00jens |title=The g factor: The science of mental ability |publisher=Praeger |year=1998 |isbn=978-0-275-96103-9 |location=Westport, CT }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Jensen |first=AR |title=Clocking the mind: Mental chronometry and individual differences |publisher=Elsevier |year=2006 |isbn=978-0-08-044939-5 |location=Amsterdam}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Jensen |first1=Arthur R |last2=Johnson |first2=Fred W |date=May–June 1994 |title=Race and sex differences in head size and IQ |journal=Intelligence |volume=18 |issue=3 |pages=309–33 |doi=10.1016/0160-2896(94)90032-9}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Kamin |first=Leon J. |date=March 2006 |title=African IQ and Mental Retardation |journal=South African Journal of Psychology |volume=36 |issue=1 |pages=1–9 |doi=10.1177/008124630603600101 |s2cid=92984213 |author-link=Leon Kamin}} | |||
*{{Cite encyclopedia |last=Kaplan |first=Jonathan Michael |url=http://www.els.net/WileyCDA/ElsArticle/refId-a0005857.html |title='Race': What Biology Can Tell Us about a Social Construct |date=January 2011 |encyclopedia=Encyclopedia of Life Sciences |isbn=978-0-470-01617-6 |doi=10.1002/9780470015902.a0005857 |access-date=2012-02-22 |archive-date=2021-05-25 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210525023933/http://www.els.net/WileyCDA/ElsArticle/refId-a0005857.html |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Kaplan |first=Jonathan Michael |date=6 February 2014 |title=Race, IQ, and the search for statistical signals associated with so-called "X"-factors: environments, racism, and the "hereditarian hypothesis" |journal=Biology & Philosophy |volume=30 |issue=1 |pages=1–17 |doi=10.1007/s10539-014-9428-0 |s2cid=85351431}} | |||
* {{cite journal |last1=Kaplan |first1=J. M. |last2=Winther |first2=R. G. |title=Prisoners of Abstraction? The Theory and Measure of Genetic Variation, and the Very Concept of 'Race' |journal=] |date=2013 |volume=7 |pages=401–412 |doi=10.1002/9780470015902.a0005857 |url=http://philpapers.org/archive/KAPPOA.14.pdf}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Kaszycka |first1=Katarzyna A. |last2=Štrkalj |first2=Goran |last3=Strzałko |first3=Jan |year=2009 |title=Current Views of European Anthropologists on Race: Influence of Educational and Ideological Background |journal=American Anthropologist |volume=111 |issue=1 |pages=43–56 |doi=10.1111/j.1548-1433.2009.01076.x}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Lieberman |first=Leonard |year=2001 |title=How "Caucasoids" got such big crania and why they shrank: from Morton to Rushton." |journal=Current Anthropology |volume=42 |issue=1 |pages=69–95 |doi=10.1086/318434 |pmid=14992214 |s2cid=224794908}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Loehlin |first=John C |title=The Handbook of Intelligence |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=2000 |editor-last=Sternberg |editor-first=Robert J |location=Cambridge |chapter=Group Differences in Intelligence}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Ludy |first=Benjamin T |title=Brief History of Modern Psychology |publisher=Wiley-Blackwell |year=2006 |isbn=978-1-4051-3206-0 |pages=188–91}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=MacEachern |first=Scott |date=March 2006 |title=Africanist archaeology and ancient IQ: racial science and cultural evolution in the twenty-first century |journal=World Archaeology |volume=38 |issue=1 |pages=72–92 |doi=10.1080/00438240500509918 |s2cid=162340124}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Mackenzie |first=Brian |date=1984 |title=Explaining race differences in IQ: The logic, the methodology, and the evidence. |journal=American Psychologist |language=en |volume=39 |issue=11 |pages=1214–1233 |doi=10.1037/0003-066x.39.11.1214}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Mackintosh |first=N. J. |url=https://archive.org/details/iqhumanintellige00mack |url-access=registration |title=IQ and Human Intelligence |publisher=Oxford University Press |year=1998 |isbn=978-0-19-852367-3 |author-link=Nicholas Mackintosh }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Mackintosh |first=N. J. |title=IQ and Human Intelligence |publisher=Oxford University Press |year=2011 |isbn=978-0-19-958559-5 |edition=second |location=Oxford}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Manly |first1=JJ |last2=Byrd |first2=DA |last3=Touradji |first3=P |last4=Stern |first4=Yaakov |year=2004 |title=Acculturation, Reading Level, and Neuropsychological Test Performance Among African American Elders |journal=Applied Neuropsychology |volume=11 |issue=1 |pages=37–46 |doi=10.1207/s15324826an1101_5 |pmid=15471745 |s2cid=30490056 |ref={{harvid|Manly et al.|2004}}}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Manly |first1=Jennifer J |last2=Jacobs |first2=Diane M |last3=Touradji |first3=Pegah |last4=Small |first4=Scott A |last5=Stern |first5=Yaakov |year=2002 |title=Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders |journal=Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society |volume=8 |issue=3 |pages=341–348 |doi=10.1017/S1355617702813157 |pmid=11939693 |s2cid=14360177 |ref={{harvid|Manly et al.|2002}}}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last1=Maltby |first1=John |title=Personality, Individual Differences and Intelligence |last2=Day |first2=Liz |last3=Macaskill |first3=Ann |publisher=] |year=2007 |isbn=978-0-13-129760-9 |pages=334–47}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=McDaniel |first=M. A. |year=2005 |title=Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence |journal=Intelligence |volume=33 |issue=4 |pages=337–346 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2004.11.005}} | |||
*<!--not peer reviewed-->{{Cite journal |last=Mickelson |first=Roslyn Arlin |year=2003 |title=When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? A Social Science Perspective |journal=Teachers College Record |volume=105 |issue=6 |pages=1052–1086 |doi=10.1111/1467-9620.00277}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Miele |first=Frank |title=Intelligence, Race and Genetics: Conversations with Arthur R. Jensen |publisher=Basic Books |year=2002 |page=256 |author-link=Frank Miele}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=MMWR |date=27 May 2005 |title=Blood Lead Levels—United States, 1999–2002 |url=https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5420a5.htm |journal=Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report |volume=54 |issue=20 |pages=513–6 |pmid=15917736 |access-date=10 September 2017 |archive-date=5 November 2019 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20191105232114/https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5420a5.htm |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Moore |first=Elsie G |date=May 1986 |title=Family socialization and the IQ test performance of traditionally and transracially adopted Black children |journal=Developmental Psychology |volume=22 |issue=3 |pages=317–26 |doi=10.1037/0012-1649.22.3.317}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Moore |first1=David S. |last2=Shenk |first2=David |date=2016-12-01 |title=The heritability fallacy |journal=Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science |language=en |volume=8 |issue=1–2 |pages=e1400 |doi=10.1002/wcs.1400 |issn=1939-5078 |pmid=27906501}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Murray |first=Charles |year=2006 |title=Changes over time in the black–white difference on mental tests: Evidence from the children of the 1979 cohort of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth |journal=Intelligence |volume=34 |issue=6 |pages=527–540 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.004 |author-link=Charles Murray (political scientist)}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Murray |first=Charles |date=July–August 2007 |title=The magnitude and components of change in the black–white IQ difference from 1920 to 1991: A birth cohort analysis of the Woodcock–Johnson standardizations |journal=Intelligence |volume=35 |issue=4 |pages=305–18 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2007.02.001}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Neisser |first=Ulric |year=2004 |title=Serious Scientists or Disgusting Racists? |journal=Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of Books |volume=49 |issue=1 |pages=5–7 |doi=10.1037/004224 |author-link=Ulric Neisser}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Neisser |first1=Ulric |last2=Boodoo |first2=Gwyneth |last3=Bouchard |first3=Thomas J Jr |last4=Boykin |first4=A. Wade |last5=Brody |first5=Nathan |last6=Ceci |first6=Stephen J |last7=Halpern |first7=Diane F |last8=Loehlin |first8=John C |last9=Perloff |first9=Robert |last10=Sternberg |first10=Robert J |last11=Urbina |first11=Susana |year=1996 |title=Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns |url=http://matt.colorado.edu/teaching/highcog/fall8/nbbbbchlpsu96.pdf |journal=American Psychologist |volume=51 |issue=2 |pages=77–101 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77 |access-date=2014-08-23 |archive-date=2015-04-20 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150420144859/http://matt.colorado.edu/teaching/highcog/fall8/nbbbbchlpsu96.pdf |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Nevid |first=Jeffrey S. |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=dO8bCgAAQBAJ |title=Essentials of Psychology: Concepts and Applications |date=2014-01-17 |publisher=Cengage Learning |isbn=978-1-285-75122-1 |language=en |access-date=2018-01-30 |archive-date=2023-03-20 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230320043711/https://books.google.com/books?id=dO8bCgAAQBAJ |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Nisbett |first=Richard |year=2005 |title=Heredity, environment, and race differences in IQ: A commentary on Rushton and Jensen |url=http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/30years/Nisbett-commentary-on-30years.pdf |journal=Psychology, Public Policy, and Law |volume=11 |issue=2 |pages=302–10 |doi=10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.302 |author-link=Richard E. Nisbett |access-date=2010-04-08 |archive-date=2011-06-05 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110605054810/http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/30years/Nisbett-commentary-on-30years.pdf |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Nisbett |first=Richard |url=https://archive.org/details/intelligencehowt00nisb |title=Intelligence and How to Get It: Why Schools and Cultures Count |publisher=W. W. Norton & Company |year=2009 |isbn=978-0-393-06505-3 }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |last3=Blair |first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens |first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |year=2012a |title=Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments |journal=American Psychologist |volume=67 |number=2 |pages=130–159 |issn=0003-066X |pmid=22233090 |doi=10.1037/a0026699}} | |||
**{{lay source |template=cite web |date=2012-05-10 |title=The latest on intelligence |url=http://www.danielwillingham.com/1/post/2012/05/the-latest-on-intelligence.html |website=Daniel Willingham}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |last3=Blair |first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens |first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |year=2012b |title=Group differences in IQ are best understood as environmental in origin |url=http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Nisbett%20(2012)%20Group.pdf |journal=American Psychologist |volume=67 |pages=503–504 |doi=10.1037/a0029772 |issn=0003-066X |pmid=22963427 |access-date=22 July 2013 |number=6 |archive-date=23 January 2015 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150123114230/http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Nisbett%20(2012)%20Group.pdf |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last1=Niu |first1=Weihua |title=The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence |last2=Brass |first2=Jillian |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=2011 |isbn=978-0-521-51806-2 |editor-last=Sternberg |editor-first=Robert J |chapter=Intelligence in Worldwide Perspective |editor-last2=Kaufmann |editor-first2=Scott Barry}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Ogbu |first=John |title=Minority Education and Caste: The American System in Cross-Cultural Perspective |publisher=Academic Press |year=1978 |isbn=978-0-125-24250-9 |location=New York}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Ogbu |first=John U |title=Cross-cultural roots of minority child development |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |year=1994 |editor-last=Greenfield |editor-first=PM |location=Hillsdale, NJ |pages= |chapter=From cultural differences to differences in cultural frames of reference |editor-last2=Cocking |editor-first2=RR |chapter-url=https://archive.org/details/crossculturalroo00gree |chapter-url-access=registration }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Olness |first=K |date=April 2003 |title=Effects on brain development leading to cognitive impairment: a worldwide epidemic |journal=Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics |volume=24 |issue=2 |pages=120–30 |doi=10.1097/00004703-200304000-00009 |pmid=12692458 |s2cid=31999992}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last1=Pickren |first1=Wade E |title=A History of Modern Psychology in Context |last2=Rutherford |first2=Alexandra |publisher=Wiley |year=2010 |author-link2=Alexandra Rutherford}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last1=Plotnik |first1=Rod |title=Introduction to Psychology |last2=Kouyoumdjian |first2=Haig |publisher=Cengage Learning |year=2011 |chapter=Intelligence}}<!--<-See "Binet's Two Warnings"--> | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Qian |first1=M |last2=Wang |first2=D |last3=Watkins |first3=WE |last4=Gebski |first4=V |last5=Yan |first5=YQ |last6=Li |first6=M |last7=Chen |first7=ZP |year=2005 |title=The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China |journal=Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition |volume=14 |issue=1 |pages=32–42 |pmid=15734706 |ref={{harvid|Qian et al.|2005}}}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Reichman |first=Nancy E. |date=2005 |title=Low birth weight and school readiness |journal=The Future of Children |volume=15 |issue=1 |pages=91–116 |doi=10.1353/foc.2005.0008 |issn=1054-8289 |pmid=16130543 |s2cid=23345980}} | |||
*{{Cite encyclopedia |year=1988 |encyclopedia=Human Abilities in Cultural Context |publisher=Cambridge University Press |last=Reuning |first=Helmut |title=Testing Bushmen in the Central Kalahari |editor-last=Irvine |editor-first=SH |pages=453–486 |doi=10.1017/CBO9780511574603.018 |isbn=978-0-511-57460-3 |url=http://ebooks.cambridge.org/chapter.jsf?bid=CBO9780511574603&cid=CBO9780511574603A028 |editor2-last=Berry |editor2-first=JW |access-date=2010-04-26 |archive-date=2011-07-25 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110725133144/http://ebooks.cambridge.org/chapter.jsf?bid=CBO9780511574603&cid=CBO9780511574603A028 |url-status=live }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Richardson |first=K |year=2004 |title=Book Review: IQ and the Wealth of Nations |journal=Heredity |volume=92 |issue=4 |pages=359–60 |doi=10.1038/sj.hdy.6800418 |doi-access=free}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Rindermann |first1=Heiner |last2=Thompson |first2=James |date=November 2013 |title=Ability rise in NAEP and narrowing ethnic gaps? |journal=Intelligence |volume=41 |issue=6 |pages=821–831 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2013.06.016 |author-link1=Heiner Rindermann}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Rose |first=Steven |year=2009 |title=Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? NO: Science and society do not benefit |journal=Nature |volume=457 |issue=7231 |pages=786–8 |bibcode=2009Natur.457..786R |doi=10.1038/457786a |pmid=19212384 |s2cid=42846614}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Roth |first1=PL |last2=Bevier |first2=CA |last3=Bobko |first3=P |last4=Switzer |first4=FS III |last5=Tyler |first5=P |year=2001 |title=Ethnic group differences in cognitive ability in employment and educational settings: A metaanalysis |journal=Personnel Psychology |volume=54 |issue=2 |pages=297–330 |citeseerx=10.1.1.372.6092 |doi=10.1111/j.1744-6570.2001.tb00094.x |ref={{harvid|Roth et al.|2001}}}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Rouvroy |first=Antoinette |title=Human genes and neoliberal governance: a Foucauldian critique |publisher=Routledge |year=2008 |isbn=978-0-415-44433-0 |page=86}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Rowe |first1=David C |date=January 2005 |editor-last=Rodgers |editor-first=Joseph |title=Under the Skin: On the Impartial Treatment of Genetic and Environmental Hypothesis of Racial Differences |journal=American Psychologist |volume=60 |issue=1 |pages=60–70 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.60 |pmid=15641922}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Rushton |first1=J. Philippe |last2=Jensen |first2=Arthur R |year=2005 |title=Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability |url=http://psychology.uwo.ca/faculty/rushtonpdfs/PPPL1.pdf |journal=Psychology, Public Policy, and Law |volume=11 |issue=2 |pages=246–8 |citeseerx=10.1.1.186.102 |doi=10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20151103215722/http://psychology.uwo.ca/faculty/rushtonpdfs/PPPL1.pdf |archive-date=2015-11-03}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Saloojee |first1=Haroon |last2=Pettifor |first2=John M |date=December 2001 |title=Iron deficiency and impaired child development |journal=BMJ |volume=323 |issue=7326 |pages=1377–8 |doi=10.1136/bmj.323.7326.1377 |pmc=1121846 |pmid=11744547}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last1=Scarr |first1=S. |title=Methods of family research: Biographies of research projects |last2=Weinberg |first2=R. A. |year=1990 |volume=1 |pages=121–151 |chapter=The nature-nurture problem revisited: The Minnesota adoption studies}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Scarr |first1=S. |last2=Weinberg |first2=R. A. |year=1976 |title=IQ test performance of black children adopted by White families |journal=American Psychologist |volume=31 |issue=10 |pages=726–739 |doi=10.1037/0003-066x.31.10.726 |s2cid=32410011 }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last1=Schacter |first1=Daniel |title=Psychology |last2=Gilbert |first2=Daniel |last3=Wegner |first3=Daniel |publisher=Worth Publishing |year=2007 |isbn=978-0-7167-5215-8 |location=New York |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/psychology0000scha_e2x8 }} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=James A. Banks |title=Encyclopedia of Race, Ethnicity and Society |publisher=SAGE |year=2008 |editor-last=Schaefer |editor-first=Richard T. |volume=2 |page=1091 |chapter=Race |ref={{harvid|Schaefer|2008}} |author-link=James A. Banks}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Serpell |first=Robert |title=Handbook of Intelligence |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=2000 |isbn=978-0-521-59648-0 |editor-last=Sternberg |editor-first=Robert J. |pages=549–577 |chapter=Intelligence and Culture}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Sheppard |first1=Leah D. |last2=Vernon |first2=Philip A. |date=February 2008 |title=Intelligence and speed of information-processing: A review of 50 years of research |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |volume=44 |issue=3 |pages=535–551 |doi=10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.015}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Shurkin |first=Joel |title=Broken Genius: The Rise and Fall of William Shockley, Creator of the Electronic Age |publisher=Macmillan |year=2006 |isbn=978-1-4039-8815-7 |location=London}} | |||
**{{lay source |template=cite magazine |author=Brian Clegg |title=Review - Broken Genius - Joel Shurkin |url=http://www.popularscience.co.uk/reviews/rev291.htm |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20061006151557/http://www.popularscience.co.uk/reviews/rev291.htm |archive-date=2006-10-06 |magazine=Popular Science}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Shuttleworth-Edwards |first1=AB |last2=Kemp |first2=RD |last3=Rust |first3=AL |last4=Muirhead |first4=JG |last5=Hartman |first5=NP |last6=Radloff |first6=SE |date=October 2004 |title=Cross-cultural effects on IQ test performance: a review and preliminary normative indications on WAIS-III test performance |journal=Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology |volume=26 |issue=7 |pages=903–20 |doi=10.1080/13803390490510824 |pmid=15742541 |s2cid=16060622 |ref={{harvid|Shuttleworth-Edwards et al.|2004}}}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Smay |first1=Diana |last2=Armelagos |first2=George |date=July 2000 |title=Galileo Wept: A Critical Assessment of the Use of Race in Forensic Anthropology |journal=Transforming Anthropology |volume=9 |issue=2 |pages=19–29 |doi=10.1525/tran.2000.9.2.19|s2cid=143942539 }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Snyderman |first1=Mark |last2=Rothman |first2=Stanley |year=1987 |title=Survey of expert opinion on intelligence and aptitude testing |journal=American Psychologist |volume=42 |issue=2 |pages=137–44 |doi=10.1037/0003-066x.42.2.137}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Sternberg |first1=Robert J |last2=Grigorenko |first2=Elena L |last3=Kidd |first3=Kenneth K |year=2005 |title=Intelligence, Race, and Genetics |url=http://medicine.yale.edu/labs/kidd/440.pdf |journal=American Psychologist |volume=60 |issue=1 |pages=46–59 |citeseerx=10.1.1.174.313 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.46 |pmid=15641921 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110720180251/http://medicine.yale.edu/labs/kidd/440.pdf |archive-date=2011-07-20 }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Sternberg |first1=R. J. |last2=Grigorenko |first2=E. L. |year=2007 |title=The Difficulty of Escaping Preconceptions in Writing an Article About the Difficulty of Escaping Preconceptions: Commentary on Hunt and Carlson (2007) |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |volume=2 |issue=2 |pages=221–223 |doi=10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00040.x |pmid=26151963 |s2cid=21096328}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Spiro |first=Jonathan P. |title=Defending the Master Race: Conservation, Eugenics, and the Legacy of Madison Grant |publisher=Univ. of Vermont Press |year=2009 |isbn=978-1-58465-715-6}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Suzuki |first1=Lisa |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |date=2005 |title=The cultural malleability of intelligence and its impact on the racial/ethnic hierarchy. |journal=Psychology, Public Policy, and Law |volume=11 |issue=2 |pages=320–327 |citeseerx=10.1.1.1022.3693 |doi=10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.320}} | |||
*<!--to identify citation in quotation in mainspace from Hunt & Carlson (2007)-->{{Cite journal |last1=Tang |first1=Hua |last2=Quertermous |first2=Tom |last3=Rodriguez |first3=Beatriz |last4=Kardia |first4=Sharon L.R. |last5=Zhu |first5=Xiaofeng |last6=Brown |first6=Andrew |last7=Pankow |first7=James S. |last8=Province |first8=Michael A. |last9=Hunt |first9=Steven C. |last10=Boerwinkle |first10=Eric |last11=Schork |first11=Nicholas J. |date=February 2005 |title=Genetic structure, self-identified race/ethnicity, and confounding in case-control association studies |journal=American Journal of Human Genetics |volume=76 |issue=2 |pages=268–75 |doi=10.1086/427888 |pmc=1196372 |pmid=15625622 |ref={{harvid|Tang et al.|2005}} |first12=Neil J. |last12=Risch}} | |||
*{{Citation |last=Templeton |first=A. R. |title=Race and Intelligence Separating Science from Myth |date=2001 |pages=31–55 |editor-last=Fish |editor-first=J. M. |contribution=The Genetic and Evolutionary Significance of Human Races |place=London |publisher=Routledge |isbn=978-0-8058-3757-5 |author-link=Alan R. Templeton}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Thompson |first1=Paul M. |last2=Cannon |first2=Tyrone D. |last3=Narr |first3=Katherine L. |last4=van Erp |first4=Theo |last5=Poutanen |first5=Veli-Pekka |last6=Huttunen |first6=Matti |last7=Lönnqvist |first7=Jouko |last8=Standertskjöld-Nordenstam |first8=Carl-Gustaf |last9=Kaprio |first9=Jaakko |last10=Khaledy |first10=Mohammad |last11=Dail |first11=Rajneesh |year=2001 |title=Genetic influences on brain structure |url=http://www.loni.ucla.edu/~thompson/MEDIA/NN/Nature_Neuro2001_genetics.pdf |journal=Nature Neuroscience |volume=4 |issue=12 |pages=1253–58 |doi=10.1038/nn758 |pmid=11694885 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110515085921/http://www.loni.ucla.edu/~thompson/MEDIA/NN/Nature_Neuro2001_genetics.pdf |archive-date=2011-05-15 |ref={{harvid|Thompson et al.|2001}} |first12=Chris I. |last12=Zoumalan |first13=Arthur W. |last13=Toga |s2cid=7132470 }} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Tizard |first1=Barbara |last2=Cooperman |first2=Oliver |last3=Joseph |first3=Anne |last4=Tizard |first4=Jack |date=June 1972 |title=Environmental effects on language development: A study of young children in long-stay residential nurseries |journal=Child Development |volume=43 |issue=2 |pages=337–58 |doi=10.2307/1127540 |jstor=1127540 |ref={{harvid|Tizard et al.|1972}}}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Tucker |first=William H |title=The Funding of Scientific Racism: Wickliffe Draper and the Pioneer Fund |publisher=University of Illinois Press |year=2002 |isbn=978-0-252-02762-8 |author-link=William H. Tucker (psychologist)}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Verney |first1=Steven P. |last2=Granholm |first2=Eric |last3=Marshall |first3=Sandra P. |last4=Malcarne |first4=Vanessa L. |last5=Saccuzzo |first5=Dennis P. |date=26 July 2016 |title=Culture-Fair Cognitive Ability Assessment |journal=Assessment |volume=12 |issue=3 |pages=303–319 |doi=10.1177/1073191105276674 |pmid=16123251 |s2cid=31024437 |ref={{harvid|Verney et al.|2005}}}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last=Vincent |first=Ken R. |date=March 1991 |title=Black/white IQ differences: Does age make the difference? |journal=Journal of Clinical Psychology |volume=47 |issue=2 |pages=266–270 |doi=10.1002/1097-4679(199103)47:2<266::aid-jclp2270470213>3.0.co;2-s |pmid=2030133}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Weinberg |first1=Richard A. |last2=Scarr |first2=Sandra |last3=Waldman |first3=Irwin D. |year=1992 |title=The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study: A Follow-Up of IQ Test Performance at Adolescence |journal=Intelligence |volume=16 |issue=1 |pages=117–35 |doi=10.1016/0160-2896(92)90028-P}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Wicherts |first1=Jelte M |last2=Borsboom |first2=Denny |last3=Dolan |first3=Conor V |year=2010 |title=Why national IQs do not support evolutionary theories of intelligence |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |volume=48 |issue=2 |pages=91–6 |doi=10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.028}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Wicherts |first1=Jelte M. |last2=Borsboom |first2=Denny |last3=Dolan |first3=Conor V. |year=2010b |title=Evolution, brain size, and the national IQ of peoples around 3000 years B.C |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |volume=48 |issue=2 |pages=104–106 |doi=10.1016/j.paid.2009.08.020}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Wicherts |first1=Jelte M. |last2=Dolan |first2=Conor V. |last3=Carlson |first3=Jerry S. |last4=van der Maas |first4=Han L.J. |date=June 2010 |title=Raven's test performance of sub-Saharan Africans: Average performance, psychometric properties, and the Flynn Effect |journal=Learning and Individual Differences |volume=20 |issue=3 |pages=135–151 |doi=10.1016/j.lindif.2009.12.001}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Wicherts |first1=Jelte M. |last2=Dolan |first2=Conor V. |last3=van der Maas |first3=Han L.J. |date=January 2010 |title=A systematic literature review of the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans |journal=Intelligence |volume=38 |issue=1 |pages=1–20 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.002}} | |||
*{{Cite journal |last1=Witelson |first1=S. F. |last2=Beresh |first2=H. |last3=Kigar |first3=D. L. |date=February 2006 |title=Intelligence and brain size in 100 postmortem brains: sex, lateralization and age factors |journal=Brain |volume=129 |issue=2 |pages=386–398 |doi=10.1093/brain/awh696 |pmid=16339797 |doi-access=free}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Wooldridge |first=Adrian |title=Measuring the Mind: Education and Psychology in England c. 1860-c. 1990 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=1995 |isbn=978-0-521-39515-1}} | |||
*{{Cite book |last=Wroe |first=Andrew |title=The Republican Party and Immigration Politics: from Proposition 187 to George W. Bush |publisher=Macmillan |year=2008 |isbn=978-0-230-60053-9 |page=294}} | |||
{{refend}} | {{refend}} | ||
{{Human intelligence topics}} | |||
== External links == | |||
{{Authority control}} | |||
{{External links|date=May 2009}} | |||
=== Collective statements === | |||
* | |||
* . ]. Adopted December 1994. | |||
* . ''Intelligence'', v24 n1 p. 13–23 January–February 1997 | |||
=== Review papers === | |||
* – | |||
* | |||
** J. Philippe Rushton & Arthur R. Jensen | |||
** Robert J. Sternberg | |||
** Linda S. Gottfredson | |||
** Richard E. Nisbett | |||
** Lisa Suzuki & Joshua Aronson | |||
** J. Philippe Rushton & Arthur R. Jensen | |||
* Richard E. Nisbett (PDF) | |||
* Charles Murray | |||
* online (page-image) version of ISBN 0-8157-4609-1 | |||
=== Others === | |||
* by ] | |||
* Race and IQ: , , by ] | |||
* | |||
* ], of Sarich’s and Miele’s book, Race: The Reality of Human Differences] | |||
* | |||
* | |||
* by ] et al. | |||
* | |||
* | |||
* | |||
* ], "This racist undercurrent in the tide of genetic research: As taboos fall away, there's a danger that denial of racial difference will be replaced with uncritical acceptance," '']''. | |||
* , ], June 2006, '']''. | |||
{{Race and sex differences}} | |||
{{DEFAULTSORT:Race And Intelligence}} | {{DEFAULTSORT:Race And Intelligence}} | ||
] | ] | ||
] | |||
] | ] | ||
] | ] | ||
] | |||
] | ] | ||
] | |||
] | ] | ||
] | ] | ||
] | ] | ||
] | |||
] | |||
] | |||
] |
Latest revision as of 06:37, 5 January 2025
Discussions and claims of differences in intelligence along racial lines
Race |
---|
History |
Society |
Race and... |
By location |
Related topics |
Discussions of race and intelligence – specifically regarding claims of differences in intelligence along racial lines – have appeared in both popular science and academic research since the modern concept of race was first introduced. With the inception of IQ testing in the early 20th century, differences in average test performance between racial groups have been observed, though these differences have fluctuated and in many cases steadily decreased over time. Complicating the issue, modern science has concluded that race is a socially constructed phenomenon rather than a biological reality, and there exist various conflicting definitions of intelligence. In particular, the validity of IQ testing as a metric for human intelligence is disputed. Today, the scientific consensus is that genetics does not explain differences in IQ test performance between groups, and that observed differences are environmental in origin.
Pseudoscientific claims of inherent differences in intelligence between races have played a central role in the history of scientific racism. The first tests showing differences in IQ scores between different population groups in the United States were the tests of United States Army recruits in World War I. In the 1920s, groups of eugenics lobbyists argued that these results demonstrated that African Americans and certain immigrant groups were of inferior intellect to Anglo-Saxon white people, and that this was due to innate biological differences. In turn, they used such beliefs to justify policies of racial segregation. However, other studies soon appeared, contesting these conclusions and arguing that the Army tests had not adequately controlled for environmental factors, such as socioeconomic and educational inequality between the groups.
Later observations of phenomena such as the Flynn effect and disparities in access to prenatal care highlighted ways in which environmental factors affect group IQ differences. In recent decades, as understanding of human genetics has advanced, claims of inherent differences in intelligence between races have been broadly rejected by scientists on both theoretical and empirical grounds.
History of the controversy
Main article: History of the race and intelligence controversy See also: Scientific racismClaims of differences in intelligence between races have been used to justify colonialism, slavery, racism, social Darwinism, and racial eugenics. Claims of intellectual inferiority were used to justify British wars and colonial campaigns in Asia. Racial thinkers such as Arthur de Gobineau in France relied crucially on the assumption that black people were innately inferior to white people in developing their ideologies of white supremacy. Even Enlightenment thinkers such as Thomas Jefferson, a slave owner, believed black people to be innately inferior to white people in physique and intellect. At the same time in the United States, prominent examples of African-American genius such the autodidact and abolitionist Frederick Douglass, the pioneering sociologist W. E. B. Du Bois, and the poet Paul Laurence Dunbar stood as high-profile counterexamples to widespread stereotypes of black intellectual inferiority. In Britain, Japan's military victory over Russia in the Russo-Japanese War began to reverse negative stereotypes of "oriental" inferiority.
Early IQ testing
The first practical intelligence test, the Binet-Simon Intelligence Test, was developed between 1905 and 1908 by Alfred Binet and Théodore Simon in France for school placement of children. Binet warned that results from his test should not be assumed to measure innate intelligence or used to label individuals permanently. Binet's test was translated into English and revised in 1916 by Lewis Terman (who introduced IQ scoring for the test results) and published under the name Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales. In 1916 Terman wrote that Mexican-Americans, African-Americans, and Native Americans have a mental "dullness seems to be racial, or at least inherent in the family stocks from which they come."
The US Army used a different set of tests developed by Robert Yerkes to evaluate draftees for World War I. Based on the Army's data, prominent psychologists and eugenicists such as Henry H. Goddard, Harry H. Laughlin, and Princeton professor Carl Brigham wrote that people from southern and eastern Europe were less intelligent than native-born Americans or immigrants from the Nordic countries, and that black Americans were less intelligent than white Americans. The results were widely publicized by a lobby of anti-immigration activists, including the conservationist and theorist of scientific racism Madison Grant, who considered the so-called Nordic race to be superior, but under threat because of immigration by "inferior breeds." In his influential work, A Study of American Intelligence, psychologist Carl Brigham used the results of the Army tests to argue for a stricter immigration policy, limiting immigration to countries considered to belong to the "Nordic race".
In the 1920s, some US states enacted eugenic laws, such as Virginia's 1924 Racial Integrity Act, which established the one-drop rule (of 'racial purity') as law. Many scientists reacted negatively to eugenicist claims linking abilities and moral character to racial or genetic ancestry. They pointed to the contribution of environment (such as speaking English as a second language) to test results. By the mid-1930s, many psychologists in the US had adopted the view that environmental and cultural factors played a dominant role in IQ test results. The psychologist Carl Brigham repudiated his own earlier arguments, explaining that he had come to realize that the tests were not a measure of innate intelligence.
Discussions of the issue in the United States, especially in the writings of Madison Grant, influenced German Nazi claims that the "Nordics" were a "master race." As American public sentiment shifted against the Germans, claims of racial differences in intelligence increasingly came to be regarded as problematic. Anthropologists such as Franz Boas, Ruth Benedict, and Gene Weltfish did much to demonstrate that claims about racial hierarchies of intelligence were unscientific. Nonetheless, a powerful eugenics and segregation lobby funded largely by textile-magnate Wickliffe Draper continued to use intelligence studies as an argument for eugenics, segregation, and anti-immigration legislation.
The Pioneer Fund and The Bell Curve
As the desegregation of the American South gained traction in the 1950s, debate about black intelligence resurfaced. Audrey Shuey, funded by Draper's Pioneer Fund, published a new analysis of Yerkes' tests, concluding that black people really were of inferior intellect to white people. This study was used by segregationists to argue that it was to the advantage of black children to be educated separately from the superior white children. In the 1960s, the debate was revived when William Shockley publicly defended the view that black children were innately unable to learn as well as white children. Arthur Jensen expressed similar opinions in his Harvard Educational Review article, "How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement?," which questioned the value of compensatory education for African-American children. He suggested that poor educational performance in such cases reflected an underlying genetic cause rather than lack of stimulation at home or other environmental factors.
Another revival of public debate followed the appearance of The Bell Curve (1994), a book by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray that supported the general viewpoint of Jensen. A statement in support of Herrnstein and Murray titled "Mainstream Science on Intelligence," was published in The Wall Street Journal with 52 signatures. The Bell Curve also led to critical responses in a statement titled "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns" of the American Psychological Association and in several books, including The Bell Curve Debate (1995), Inequality by Design (1996) and a second edition of The Mismeasure of Man (1996) by Stephen Jay Gould.
Some of the authors proposing genetic explanations for group differences have received funding from the Pioneer Fund, which was headed by J. Philippe Rushton until his death in 2012. Arthur Jensen, who jointly with Rushton published a 2005 review article arguing that the difference in average IQs between blacks and whites is partly due to genetics, received $1.1 million in grants from the Pioneer Fund. According to Ashley Montagu, "The University of California's Arthur Jensen, cited twenty-three times in The Bell Curve's bibliography, is the book's principal authority on the intellectual inferiority of blacks."
The Southern Poverty Law Center lists the Pioneer Fund as a hate group, citing the fund's history, its funding of race and intelligence research, and its connections with racist individuals. Other researchers have criticized the Pioneer Fund for promoting scientific racism, eugenics and white supremacy.
Conceptual issues
Intelligence and IQ
Main articles: Human intelligence, Intelligence quotient, and G factor (psychometrics)The concept of intelligence and the degree to which intelligence is measurable are matters of debate. There is no consensus about how to define intelligence; nor is it universally accepted that it is something that can be meaningfully measured by a single figure. A recurring criticism is that different societies value and promote different kinds of skills and that the concept of intelligence is therefore culturally variable and cannot be measured by the same criteria in different societies. Consequently, some critics argue that it makes no sense to propose relationships between intelligence and other variables.
Correlations between scores on various types of IQ tests led English psychologist Charles Spearman to propose in 1904 the existence of an underlying factor, which he referred to as "g" or "general intelligence", a trait which is supposed to be innate. Another proponent of this view is Arthur Jensen. This view, however, has been contradicted by a number of studies showing that education and changes in environment can significantly improve IQ test results.
Other psychometricians have argued that, whether or not there is such a thing as a general intelligence factor, performance on tests relies crucially on knowledge acquired through prior exposure to the types of tasks that such tests contain. This means that comparisons of test scores between persons with widely different life experiences and cognitive habits do not reveal their relative innate potentials.
Race
Main articles: Race (human categorization) and Race and geneticsThe consensus view among geneticists, biologists and anthropologists is that race is a sociopolitical phenomenon rather than a biological one, a view supported by considerable genetics research. The current mainstream view is that race is a social construction based on folk ideologies that construct groups based on social disparities and superficial physical characteristics. A 2023 consensus report from the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine stated: "In humans, race is a socially constructed designation, a misleading and harmful surrogate for population genetic differences, and has a long history of being incorrectly identified as the major genetic reason for phenotypic differences between groups."
The concept of human "races" as natural and separate divisions within the human species has also been rejected by the American Anthropological Association. The official position of the AAA, adopted in 1998, is that advances in scientific knowledge have made it "clear that human populations are not unambiguous, clearly demarcated, biologically distinct groups" and that "any attempt to establish lines of division among biological populations both arbitrary and subjective." A more recent statement from the American Association of Physical Anthropologists (2019) declares that "Race does not provide an accurate representation of human biological variation. It was never accurate in the past, and it remains inaccurate when referencing contemporary human populations. Humans are not divided biologically into distinct continental types or racial genetic clusters."
Anthropologists such as C. Loring Brace, the philosophers Jonathan Kaplan and Rasmus Winther, and the geneticist Joseph Graves, have argued that the cluster structure of genetic data is dependent on the initial hypotheses of the researcher and the influence of these hypotheses on the choice of populations to sample. When one samples continental groups, the clusters become continental, but if one had chosen other sampling patterns, the clustering would be different. Weiss and Fullerton have noted that if one sampled only Icelanders, Mayans and Maoris, three distinct clusters would form and all other populations could be described as being clinally composed of admixtures of Maori, Icelandic and Mayan genetic materials. Kaplan and Winther conclude that while racial groups are characterized by different allele frequencies, this does not mean that racial classification is a natural taxonomy of the human species, because multiple other genetic patterns can be found in human populations that crosscut racial distinctions. Moreover, the genomic data underdetermines whether one wishes to see subdivisions (i.e., splitters) or a continuum (i.e., lumpers). Under Kaplan and Winther's view, racial groupings are objective social constructions (see Mills 1998) that have conventional biological reality only insofar as the categories are chosen and constructed for pragmatic scientific reasons. Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd (2005) argue that the social construction of race derives not from any valid scientific basis but rather "from people's desire to classify."
In studies of human intelligence, race is almost always determined using self-reports rather than analyses of genetic characteristics. According to psychologist David Rowe, self-report is the preferred method for racial classification in studies of racial differences because classification based on genetic markers alone ignore the "cultural, behavioral, sociological, psychological, and epidemiological variables" that distinguish racial groups. Hunt and Carlson disagreed, writing that "Nevertheless, self-identification is a surprisingly reliable guide to genetic composition," citing a study by Tang et al. (2005). Sternberg and Grigorenko disputed Hunt and Carlson's interpretation of Tang's results as supporting the view that racial divisions are biological; rather, "Tang et al.'s point was that ancient geographic ancestry rather than current residence is associated with self-identification and not that such self-identification provides evidence for the existence of biological race."
Group differences
The study of human intelligence is one of the most controversial topics in psychology, in part because of difficulty reaching agreement about the meaning of intelligence and objections to the assumption that intelligence can be meaningfully measured by IQ tests. Claims that there are innate differences in intelligence between racial and ethnic groups—which go back at least to the 19th century—have been criticized for relying on specious assumptions and research methods and for serving as an ideological framework for discrimination and racism.
In a 2012 study of tests of different components of intelligence, Hampshire et al. expressed disagreement with the view of Jensen and Rushton that genetic factors must play a role in IQ differences between races, stating that "it remains unclear ... whether population differences in intelligence test scores are driven by heritable factors or by other correlated demographic variables such as socioeconomic status, education level, and motivation. More relevantly, it is questionable whether relate to a unitary intelligence factor, as opposed to a bias in testing paradigms toward particular components of a more complex intelligence construct." According to Jackson and Weidman,
There are a number of reasons why the genetic argument for race differences in intelligence has not won many adherents in the scientific community. First, even taken on its own terms, the case made by Jensen and his followers did not hold up to scrutiny. Second, the rise of population genetics undercut the claims for a genetic cause of intelligence. Third, the new understanding of institutional racism offered a better explanation for the existence of differences in IQ scores between the races.
Test scores
Main article: Achievement gap in the United StatesIn the United States, Asians on average score higher than White people, who tend to score higher than Hispanics, who tend to score higher than African Americans. Much greater variation in IQ scores exists within each ethnic group than between them. A 2001 meta-analysis of the results of 6,246,729 participants tested for cognitive ability or aptitude found a difference in average scores between black people and white people of 1.1 standard deviations. Consistent results were found for college and university application tests such as the Scholastic Aptitude Test (N = 2.4 million) and Graduate Record Examination (N = 2.3 million), as well as for tests of job applicants in corporate settings (N = 0.5 million) and in the military (N = 0.4 million).
In response to the controversial 1994 book The Bell Curve, the American Psychological Association (APA) formed a task-force of eleven experts, which issued a report "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns" in 1996. Regarding group differences, the report reaffirmed the consensus that differences within groups are much wider than differences between groups, and that claims of ethnic differences in intelligence should be scrutinized carefully, as such claims had been used to justify racial discrimination. The report also acknowledged problems with the racial categories used, as these categories are neither consistently applied, nor homogeneous (see Race and ethnicity in the United States).
In the UK, some African groups have higher average educational attainment and standardized test scores than the overall population. In 2010–2011, white British pupils were 2.3% less likely to have gained 5 A*–C grades at GCSE than the national average, whereas the likelihood was 21.8% above average for those of Nigerian origin, 5.5% above average for those of Ghanaian origin, and 1.4% above average for those of Sierra Leonian origin. For the two other African ethnic groups on which data was available, the likelihood was 23.7% below average for those of Somali origin and 35.3% below average for those of Congolese origin. In 2014, Black-African pupils of 11 language groups were more likely to pass Key Stage 2 Maths 4+ in England than the national average. Overall, the average pass rate by ethnicity was 86.5% for white British (N = 395,787), whereas it was 85.6% for Black-Africans (N = 18,497). Nevertheless, several Black-African language groups, including Yoruba, Igbo, Hausa, Akan, Ga, Swahili, Edo, Ewe, Amharic speakers, and English-speaking Africans, each had an average pass rate above the white British average (total N = 9,314), with the Hausa, Igbo, Yoruba, and Amhara having averages above 90% (N = 2,071). In 2017–2018, the percentage of pupils getting a strong pass (grade 5 or above) in the English and maths GCSE (in Key Stage 4) was 42.7% for whites (N = 396,680) and 44.3% for Black-Africans (N = 18,358).
Flynn effect and the closing gap
Main article: Flynn effectThe 'Flynn effect' — a term coined after researcher James R. Flynn — refers to the substantial rise in raw IQ test scores observed in many parts of the world during the 20th century. In the United States, the increase was continuous and approximately linear from the earliest years of testing to about 1998 when the gains stopped and some tests even showed decreasing test scores. For example, the average scores of black people on some IQ tests in 1995 were the same as the scores of white people in 1945. As one pair of academics phrased it, "the typical African American today probably has a slightly higher IQ than the grandparents of today's average white American."
Flynn himself argued that the dramatic changes having taken place between one just generation and the next pointed strongly at an environmental explanation, and that it is highly unlikely that genetic factors could have accounted for the increasing scores. The Flynn effect, along with Flynn's analysis, continues to hold significance in the context of the black/white IQ gap debate, demonstrating the potential for environmental factors to influence IQ test scores by as much as 1 standard deviation, a scale of change that had previously been doubted.
A distinct but related observation has been the gradual narrowing of the American black-white IQ gap in the last decades of the 20th century, as black test-takers increased their average scores relative to white test-takers. For instance, Vincent reported in 1991 that the black–white IQ gap was decreasing among children, but that it was remaining constant among adults. Similarly, a 2006 study by Dickens and Flynn estimated that the difference between mean scores of black people and white people closed by about 5 or 6 IQ points between 1972 and 2002, a reduction of about one-third. In the same period, the educational achievement disparity also diminished. Reviews by Flynn and Dickens, Mackintosh, and Nisbett et al. accept the gradual closing of the gap as a fact. Flynn and Dickens summarize this trend, stating, "The constancy of the Black-White IQ gap is a myth and therefore cannot be cited as evidence that the racial IQ gap is genetic in origin."
Environmental factors
Health and nutrition
Main article: Impact of health on intelligenceEnvironmental factors including childhood lead exposure, low rates of breast feeding, and poor nutrition are significantly correlated with poor cognitive development and functioning. For example, childhood exposure to lead — associated with homes in poorer areas — correlates with an average IQ drop of 7 points, and iodine deficiency causes a decline, on average, of 12 IQ points. Such impairments may sometimes be permanent, but in some cases they be partially or wholly compensated for by later growth.
The first two years of life are critical for malnutrition, the consequences of which are often irreversible and include poor cognitive development, educability, and future economic productivity. Mackintosh points out that, for American black people, infant mortality is about twice as high as for white people, and low birth weight is twice as prevalent. At the same time, white mothers are twice as likely to breastfeed their infants, and breastfeeding is directly correlated with IQ for low-birth-weight infants. In this way, a wide number of health-related factors which influence IQ are unequally distributed between the two groups.
The Copenhagen consensus in 2004 stated that lack of both iodine and iron has been implicated in impaired brain development, and this can affect enormous numbers of people: it is estimated that one-third of the total global population is affected by iodine deficiency. In developing countries, it is estimated that 40% of children aged four and under have anaemia because of insufficient iron in their diets.
Other scholars have found that simply the standard of nutrition has a significant effect on population intelligence, and that the Flynn effect may be caused by increasing nutrition standards across the world. James Flynn has himself argued against this view.
Some recent research has argued that the retardation caused in brain development by infectious diseases, many of which are more prevalent in non-white populations, may be an important factor in explaining the differences in IQ between different regions of the world. The findings of this research, showing the correlation between IQ, race and infectious diseases was also shown to apply to the IQ gap in the US, suggesting that this may be an important environmental factor.
A 2013 meta-analysis by the World Health Organization found that, after controlling for maternal IQ, breastfeeding was associated with IQ gains of 2.19 points. The authors suggest that this relationship is causal but state that the practical significance of this gain is debatable; however, they highlight one study suggesting an association between breastfeeding and academic performance in Brazil, where "breastfeeding duration does not present marked variability by socioeconomic position." Colen and Ramey (2014) similarly find that controlling for sibling comparisons within families, rather than between families, reduces the correlation between breastfeeding status and WISC IQ scores by nearly a third, but further find the relationship between breastfeeding duration and WISC IQ scores to be insignificant. They suggest that "much of the beneficial long-term effects typically attributed to breastfeeding, per se, may primarily be due to selection pressures into infant feeding practices along key demographic characteristics such as race and socioeconomic status." Reichman estimates that no more than 3 to 4% of the black–white IQ gap can be explained by black–white disparities in low birth weight.
Education
Several studies have proposed that a large part of the gap in IQ test performance can be attributed to differences in quality of education. Racial discrimination in education has been proposed as one possible cause of differences in educational quality between races. According to a paper by Hala Elhoweris, Kagendo Mutua, Negmeldin Alsheikh and Pauline Holloway, teachers' referral decisions for students to participate in gifted and talented educational programs were influenced in part by the students' ethnicity.
The Abecedarian Early Intervention Project, an intensive early childhood education project, was also able to bring about an average IQ gain of 4.4 points at age 21 in the black children who participated in it compared to controls. Arthur Jensen agreed that the Abecedarian project demonstrated that education can have a significant effect on IQ, but also declared his view that no educational program thus far had been able to reduce the black–white IQ gap by more than a third, and that differences in education are thus unlikely to be its only cause.
A series of studies by Joseph Fagan and Cynthia Holland measured the effect of prior exposure to the kind of cognitive tasks posed in IQ tests on test performance. Assuming that the IQ gap was the result of lower exposure to tasks using the cognitive functions usually found in IQ tests among African American test takers, they prepared a group of African Americans in this type of tasks before taking an IQ test. The researchers found that there was no subsequent difference in performance between the African-Americans and white test takers. Daley and Onwuegbuzie conclude that Fagan and Holland demonstrate that "differences in knowledge between black people and white people for intelligence test items can be erased when equal opportunity is provided for exposure to the information to be tested". A similar argument is made by David Marks who argues that IQ differences correlate well with differences in literacy suggesting that developing literacy skills through education causes an increase in IQ test performance.
A 2003 study found that two variables—stereotype threat and the degree of educational attainment of children's fathers—partially explained the black–white gap in cognitive ability test scores, undermining the hereditarian view that they stemmed from immutable genetic factors.
Socioeconomic environment
Different aspects of the socioeconomic environment in which children are raised have been shown to correlate with part of the IQ gap, but they do not account for the entire gap. According to a 2006 review, these factors account for slightly less than half of one standard deviation.
Other research has focused on different causes of variation within low socioeconomic status (SES) and high SES groups. In the US, among low SES groups, genetic differences account for a smaller proportion of the variance in IQ than among high SES populations. Such effects are predicted by the bioecological hypothesis—that genotypes are transformed into phenotypes through nonadditive synergistic effects of the environment. Nisbett et al. (2012a) suggest that high SES individuals are more likely to be able to develop their full biological potential, whereas low SES individuals are likely to be hindered in their development by adverse environmental conditions. The same review also points out that adoption studies generally are biased towards including only high and high middle SES adoptive families, meaning that they will tend to overestimate average genetic effects. They also note that studies of adoption from lower-class homes to middle-class homes have shown that such children experience a 12 to 18 point gain in IQ relative to children who remain in low SES homes. A 2015 study found that environmental factors (namely, family income, maternal education, maternal verbal ability/knowledge, learning materials in the home, parenting factors, child birth order, and child birth weight) accounted for the black–white gap in cognitive ability test scores.
Test bias
A number of studies have reached the conclusion that IQ tests may be biased against certain groups. The validity and reliability of IQ scores obtained from outside the United States and Europe have been questioned, in part because of the inherent difficulty of comparing IQ scores between cultures. Several researchers have argued that cultural differences limit the appropriateness of standard IQ tests in non-industrialized communities.
A 1996 report by the American Psychological Association states that intelligence can be difficult to compare across cultures, and notes that differing familiarity with test materials can produce substantial differences in test results; it also says that tests are accurate predictors of future achievement for black and white Americans, and are in that sense unbiased. The view that tests accurately predict future educational attainment is reinforced by Nicholas Mackintosh in his 1998 book IQ and Human Intelligence, and by a 1999 literature review by Brown, Reynolds & Whitaker (1999).
James R. Flynn, surveying studies on the topic, notes that the weight and presence of many test questions depends on what sorts of information and modes of thinking are culturally valued.
Stereotype threat and minority status
Main article: Stereotype threatStereotype threat is the fear that one's behavior will confirm an existing stereotype of a group with which one identifies or by which one is defined; this fear may in turn lead to an impairment of performance. Testing situations that highlight the fact that intelligence is being measured tend to lower the scores of individuals from racial-ethnic groups who already score lower on average or are expected to score lower. Stereotype threat conditions cause larger than expected IQ differences among groups. Psychometrician Nicholas Mackintosh considers that there is little doubt that the effects of stereotype threat contribute to the IQ gap between black people and white people.
A large number of studies have shown that systemically disadvantaged minorities, such as the African American minority of the United States, generally perform worse in the educational system and in intelligence tests than the majority groups or less disadvantaged minorities such as immigrant or "voluntary" minorities. The explanation of these findings may be that children of caste-like minorities, due to the systemic limitations of their prospects of social advancement, do not have "effort optimism", i.e. they do not have the confidence that acquiring the skills valued by majority society, such as those skills measured by IQ tests, is worthwhile. They may even deliberately reject certain behaviors that are seen as "acting white." Research published in 1997 indicates that part of the black–white gap in cognitive ability test scores is due to racial differences in test motivation.
Some researchers have suggested that stereotype threat should not be interpreted as a factor in real-life performance gaps, and have raised the possibility of publication bias. Other critics have focused on correcting what they claim are misconceptions of early studies showing a large effect. However, numerous meta-analyses and systematic reviews have shown significant evidence for the effects of stereotype threat, though the phenomenon defies over-simplistic characterization. For instance, one meta-analysis found that with female subjects "subtle threat-activating cues produced the largest effect, followed by blatant and moderately explicit cues" while with minorities "moderately explicit stereotype threat-activating cues produced the largest effect, followed by blatant and subtle cues".
Some researchers have argued that studies of stereotype threat may in fact systematically under-represent its effects, since such studies measure "only that portion of psychological threat that research has identified and remedied. To the extent that unidentified or unremedied psychological threats further undermine performance, the results underestimate the bias."
Research into possible genetic factors
See also: Heritability of IQAlthough IQ differences between individuals have been shown to have a large hereditary component, it does not follow that mean group-level disparities (between-group differences) in IQ necessarily have a genetic basis. The scientific consensus is that there is no evidence for a genetic component behind IQ differences between racial groups. Growing evidence indicates that environmental factors, not genetic ones, explain the racial IQ gap.
Genetics of race and intelligence
Main article: Race and geneticsGeneticist Alan R. Templeton argued that the question about the possible genetic effects on the test score gap is muddled by the general focus on "race" rather than on populations defined by gene frequency or by geographical proximity, and by the general insistence on phrasing the question in terms of heritability. Templeton pointed out that racial groups neither represent sub-species nor distinct evolutionary lineages, and that therefore there is no basis for making claims about the general intelligence of races. He argued that, for these reasons, the search for possible genetic influences on the black–white test score gap is a priori flawed, because there is no genetic material shared by all Africans or by all Europeans. Mackintosh (2011), on the other hand, argued that by using genetic cluster analysis to correlate gene frequencies with continental populations it might be possible to show that African populations have a higher frequency of certain genetic variants that contribute to differences in average intelligence. Such a hypothetical situation could hold without all Africans carrying the same genes or belonging to a single evolutionary lineage. According to Mackintosh, a biological basis for the observed gap in IQ test performance thus cannot be ruled out on a priori grounds.
Hunt (2010, p. 447) noted that "no genes related to difference in cognitive skills have across the various racial and ethnic groups have ever been discovered. The argument for genetic differences has been carried forward largely by circumstantial evidence. Of course, tomorrow afternoon genetic mechanisms producing racial and ethnic differences in intelligence might be discovered, but there have been a lot of investigations, and tomorrow has not come for quite some time now." Mackintosh (2011, p. 344) concurred, noting that while several environmental factors have been shown to influence the IQ gap, the evidence for a genetic influence has been negligible. A 2012 review by Nisbett et al. (2012a) concluded that the entire IQ gap can be explained by known environmental factors, and Mackintosh found this view to be plausible.
More recent research attempting to identify genetic loci associated with individual-level differences in IQ has yielded promising results, which led the editorial board of Nature to issue a statement differentiating this research from the "racist" pseudoscience which it acknowledged has dogged intelligence research since its inception. It characterized the idea of genetically determined differences in intelligence between races as definitively false. Analysis of polygenic scores sampled from the 1000 Genomes Project has likewise found no evidence that intelligence was under diversifying selection in Africans and Europeans, suggesting that genetic differences make up a negligible component of the observed Black-White gap in IQ.
Heritability within and between groups
Twin studies of intelligence have reported high heritability values. However, these studies have been criticized for being based on questionable assumptions. When used in the context of human behavior genetics, the term "heritability" can be misleading, as it does not necessarily convey information about the relative importance of genetic or environmental factors on the development of a given trait, nor does it convey the extent to which that trait is genetically determined. Arguments in support of a genetic explanation of racial differences in IQ are sometimes fallacious. For instance, hereditarians have sometimes cited the failure of known environmental factors to account for such differences, or the high heritability of intelligence within races, as evidence that racial differences in IQ are genetic.
Psychometricians have found that intelligence is substantially heritable within populations, with 30–50% of variance in IQ scores in early childhood being attributable to genetic factors in analyzed US populations, increasing to 75–80% by late adolescence. In biology heritability is defined as the ratio of variation attributable to genetic differences in an observable trait to the trait's total observable variation. The heritability of a trait describes the proportion of variation in the trait that is attributable to genetic factors within a particular population. A heritability of 1 indicates that variation correlates fully with genetic variation and a heritability of 0 indicates that there is no correlation between the trait and genes at all. In psychological testing, heritability tends to be understood as the degree of correlation between the results of a test taker and those of their biological parents. However, since high heritability is simply a correlation between child and parents, it does not describe the causes of heritability which in humans can be either genetic or environmental.
Therefore, a high heritability measure does not imply that a trait is genetic or unchangeable. In addition, environmental factors that affect all group members equally will not be measured by heritability, and the heritability of a trait may also change over time in response to changes in the distribution of genetic and environmental factors. High heritability does not imply that all of the heritability is genetically determined; rather, it can also be due to environmental differences that affect only a certain genetically defined group (indirect heritability).
The figure to the right demonstrates how heritability works. In each of the two gardens the difference between tall and short cornstalks is 100% heritable, as cornstalks that are genetically disposed for growing tall will become taller than those without this disposition. But the difference in height between the cornstalks to the left and those on the right is 100% environmental, as it is due to different nutrients being supplied to the two gardens. Hence, the causes of differences within a group and between groups may not be the same, even when looking at traits that are highly heritable.
Spearman's hypothesis
Main article: Spearman's hypothesisSpearman's hypothesis states that the magnitude of the black–white difference in tests of cognitive ability depends entirely or mainly on the extent to which a test measures general mental ability, or g. The hypothesis was first formalized by Arthur Jensen, who devised the statistical "method of correlated vectors" to test it. If Spearman's hypothesis holds true, then the cognitive tasks that have the highest g-load are the tasks in which the gap between black and white test takers are greatest. Jensen and Rushton took this to show that the cause of g and the cause of the gap are the same—in their view, genetic differences.
Mackintosh (2011, pp. 338–39) acknowledges that Jensen and Rushton showed a modest correlation between g-loading, heritability, and the test score gap, but does not agree that this demonstrates a genetic origin of the gap. Mackintosh argues that it is exactly those tests that Rushton and Jensen consider to have the highest g-loading and heritability, such as the Wechsler test, that have seen the greatest increases in black performance due to the Flynn effect. This likely suggests that they are also the most sensitive to environmental changes, which undermines Jensen's argument that the black–white gap is most likely caused by genetic factors. Nisbett et al. (2012a, p. 146) make the same point, noting also that the increase in the IQ scores of black test takers necessarily indicates an increase in g.
James Flynn argued that his findings undermine Spearman's hypothesis. In a 2006 study, he and William Dickens found that between 1972 and 2002 "The standard measure of the g gap between Blacks and Whites declined virtually in tandem with the IQ gap." Flynn also criticized Jensen's basic assumption that a correlation between g-loading and test score gap implies a genetic cause for the gap. In a 2014 suite of meta-analyses, along with co-authors Jan te Nijenhuis and Daniel Metzen, he showed that the same negative correlation between IQ gains and g-loading obtains for cognitive deficits of known environmental cause: iodine deficiency, prenatal cocaine exposure, fetal alcohol syndrome, and traumatic brain injury.
Adoption studies
A number of IQ studies have been done on the effect of similar rearing conditions on children from different races. The hypothesis is that this can be determined by investigating whether black children adopted into white families demonstrated gains in IQ test scores relative to black children reared in black families. Depending on whether their test scores are more similar to their biological or adoptive families, that could be interpreted as supporting either a genetic or an environmental hypothesis. Critiques of such studies question whether the environment of black children—even when raised in white families—is truly comparable to the environment of white children. Several reviews of the adoption study literature have suggested that it is probably impossible to avoid confounding biological and environmental factors in this type of study. Another criticism by Nisbett et al. (2012a, pp. 134) is that adoption studies on the whole tend to be carried out in a restricted set of environments, mostly in the medium-high SES range, where heritability is higher than in the low-SES range.
The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study (1976) examined the IQ test scores of 122 adopted children and 143 nonadopted children reared by advantaged white families. The children were restudied ten years later. The study found higher IQ for white people compared to black people, both at age 7 and age 17. Acknowledging the existence of confounding factors, Scarr and Weinberg, the authors of the original study, did not consider that it provided support for either the hereditarian or environmentalist view.
Three other studies lend support to environmental explanations of group IQ differences:
- Eyferth (1961) studied the out-of-wedlock children of black and white soldiers stationed in Germany after World War II who were then raised by white German mothers in what has become known as the Eyferth study. He found no significant differences in average IQ between groups.
- Tizard et al. (1972) studied black (West Indian), white, and mixed-race children raised in British long-stay residential nurseries. Two out of three tests found no significant differences. One test found higher scores for non-white people.
- Moore (1986) compared black and mixed-race children adopted by either black or white middle-class families in the United States. Moore observed that 23 black and interracial children raised by white parents had a significantly higher mean score than 23 age-matched children raised by black parents (117 vs 104), and argued that differences in early socialization explained these differences.
Frydman and Lynn (1989) showed a mean IQ of 119 for Korean infants adopted by Belgian families. After correcting for the Flynn effect, the IQ of the adopted Korean children was still 10 points higher than that of the Belgian children.
Reviewing the evidence from adoption studies, Mackintosh finds that environmental and genetic variables remain confounded and considers evidence from adoption studies inconclusive, and fully compatible with a 100% environmental explanation. Similarly, Drew Thomas argues that race differences in IQ that appear in adoption studies are in fact an artifact of methodology, and that East Asian IQ advantages and black IQ disadvantages disappear when this is controlled for.
Racial admixture studies
Most people have ancestry from different geographical regions. In particular, African Americans typically have ancestors from both Africa and Europe, with, on average, 20% of their genome inherited from European ancestors. If racial IQ gaps have a partially genetic basis, one might expect black people with a higher degree of European ancestry to score higher on IQ tests than black people with less European ancestry, because the genes inherited from European ancestors would likely include some genes with a positive effect on IQ. Geneticist Alan Templeton has argued that an experiment based on the Mendelian "common garden" design, where specimens with different hybrid compositions are subjected to the same environmental influences, are the only way to definitively show a causal relation between genes and group differences in IQ. Summarizing the findings of admixture studies, he concludes that they have shown no significant correlation between any cognitive ability and the degree of African or European ancestry.
Studies have employed different ways of measuring or approximating relative degrees of ancestry from Africa and Europe. Some studies have used skin color as a measure, and others have used blood groups. Loehlin (2000) surveys the literature and argues that the blood groups studies may be seen as providing some support to the genetic hypothesis, even though the correlation between ancestry and IQ was quite low. He finds that studies by Eyferth (1961), Willerman, Naylor & Myrianthopoulos (1970) did not find a correlation between degree of African/European ancestry and IQ. The latter study did find a difference based on the race of the mother, with children of white mothers with black fathers scoring higher than children of black mothers and white fathers. Loehlin considers that such a finding is compatible with either a genetic or an environmental cause. All in all Loehlin finds admixture studies inconclusive and recommends more research.
Reviewing the evidence from admixture studies Hunt (2010) considers it to be inconclusive because of too many uncontrolled variables. Mackintosh (2011, p. 338) quotes a statement by Nisbett (2009) to the effect that admixture studies have not provided a shred of evidence in favor of a genetic basis for the IQ gap.
Mental chronometry
Main article: Mental chronometryMental chronometry measures the elapsed time between the presentation of a sensory stimulus and the subsequent behavioral response by the participant. These studies have shown inconsistent results when comparing black and white populations groups, with some studies showing whites outperforming blacks, and others showing blacks outperforming whites.
Arthur Jensen argued that this reaction time (RT) is a measure of the speed and efficiency with which the brain processes information, and that scores on most types of RT tasks tend to correlate with scores on standard IQ tests as well as with g. Nisbett argues that some studies have found correlations closer to 0.2, and that a correlation is not always found. Nisbett points to the Jensen & Whang (1993) study in which a group of Chinese Americans had longer reaction times than a group of European Americans, despite having higher IQs. Nisbett also mentions findings in Flynn (1991) and Deary (2001) suggesting that movement time (the measure of how long it takes a person to move a finger after making the decision to do so) correlates with IQ just as strongly as reaction time, and that average movement time is faster for black people than for white people. Mackintosh (2011, p. 339) considers reaction time evidence unconvincing and comments that other cognitive tests that also correlate well with IQ show no disparity at all, for example the habituation/dishabituation test. He further comments that studies show that rhesus monkeys have shorter reaction times than American college students, suggesting that different reaction times may not tell us anything useful about intelligence.
Brain size
Main article: Brain sizeA number of studies have reported a moderate statistical correlation between differences in IQ and brain size between individuals in the same group. Some scholars have reported differences in average brain sizes between racial groups, although this is unlikely to be a good measure of IQ as brain size also differs between men and women, but without significant differences in IQ. At the same time newborn black children have the same average brain size as white children, suggesting that the difference in average size could be accounted for by differences in environment. Several environmental factors that reduce brain size have been demonstrated to disproportionately affect black children.
Archaeological data
Archaeological evidence does not support claims by Rushton and others that black people's cognitive ability was inferior to white people's during prehistoric times.
Policy relevance and ethics
Main article: Intelligence and public policyThe ethics of research on race and intelligence has long been a subject of debate: in a 1996 report of the American Psychological Association; in guidelines proposed by Gray and Thompson and by Hunt and Carlson; and in two editorials in Nature in 2009 by Steven Rose and by Stephen J. Ceci and Wendy M. Williams.
Steven Rose maintains that the history of eugenics makes this field of research difficult to reconcile with current ethical standards for science. On the other hand, James R. Flynn has argued that had there been a ban on research on possibly poorly conceived ideas, much valuable research on intelligence testing (including his own discovery of the Flynn effect) would not have occurred.
Many have argued for increased interventions in order to close the gaps. Flynn writes that "America will have to address all the aspects of black experience that are disadvantageous, beginning with the regeneration of inner city neighborhoods and their schools." Especially in developing nations, society has been urged to take on the prevention of cognitive impairment in children as a high priority. Possible preventable causes include malnutrition, infectious diseases such as meningitis, parasites, cerebral malaria, in utero drug and alcohol exposure, newborn asphyxia, low birth weight, head injuries, lead poisoning and endocrine disorders.
See also
References
Notes
Citations
- ^ Mercer, Jonathan (October 1, 2023). "Racism, Stereotypes, and War". direct.mit.edu. Journal of International Security. Retrieved 2024-02-04.
- Jackson & Weidman 2004, p. 23.
- Stewart, Roderick M. 1999. "The Claims of Frederick Douglass Philosophically Considered." Pp. 155–56 in Frederick Douglass: A Critical Reader, edited by B. E. Lawson and F. M. Kirkland. Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-631-20578-4. "Moreover, though he does not make the point explicitly, again the very fact that Douglass is ably disputing this argument on this occasion celebrating a select few's intellect and will (or moral character)—this fact constitutes a living counterexample to the narrowness of the pro-slavery definition of humans."
- Marable, Manning (2011), Living Black History: How Reimagining the African-American Past Can Remake America's Racial Future, p. 96. ISBN 978-0-465-04395-8.
- Tonooka, Chika (2017). "Reverse Emulation and the Cult of Japanese Efficiency in Edwardian Britain". The Historical Journal. 60 (1): 95–119. doi:10.1017/S0018246X15000539. ISSN 0018-246X. JSTOR 26343378. S2CID 162698331.
- Plotnik & Kouyoumdjian 2011.
- Terman, Lewis (1916). The Measurement Of Intelligence. Houghton, Mifflin and Company. p. 91. OCLC 557712625.
- Jackson & Weidman 2004, p. 116.
- Jackson & Weidman 2004, pp. 116, 309.
- Pickren & Rutherford 2010, p. 163.
- Jackson & Weidman 2004, p. 145.
- Spiro 2009.
- Ludy 2006
- Jackson & Weidman 2004, pp. 130–32.
- ^ Tucker 2002.
- Jackson 2005.
- Shurkin 2006.
- Jensen 1969, pp. 1–123.
- Panofsky, Aaron (2014). Misbehaving Science. Controversy and the Development of Behavior Genetics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0-226-05831-3.
- Alland 2002, pp. 79–80.
- Herrnstein & Murray 1994.
- ^ Maltby, Day & Macaskill 2007
- Mackintosh 1998
- Graves 2002a.
- Graves 2002b.
- Grossman & Kaufman 2001
- Adam, Miller (1994). "The Pioneer Fund: Bankrolling the Professors of Hate". The Journal of Blacks in Higher Education (6): 58–61. doi:10.2307/2962466. JSTOR 2962466.
- Blakemore, Bill; Jennings, Peter; Nissen, Beth (November 22, 1994). "The Bell Curve and the Pioneer Fund". ABC World News Tonight. ABC News. Archived from the original on March 3, 2016. Retrieved May 1, 2020. Vanderbilt Television News Archive : ABC Evening News for Tuesday, Nov 22, 1994. Headline: American Agenda (Intelligence) Archived January 3, 2016, at the Wayback Machine
- Montagu, Ashley (2002). Race and IQ (2 ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-510221-5.
- Berlet 2003.
- Pioneer Fund Board Archived 2011-05-25 at the Wayback Machine
- Falk 2008, p. 18
- Wroe 2008, p. 81
- ^ Schacter, Gilbert & Wegner 2007, pp. 350–1
- ^ Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd 2005
- Deary, I. J.; Lawn, M.; Bartholomew, D. J. (2008). ""A conversation between Charles Spearman, Godfrey Thomson, and Edward L. Thorndike: The International Examinations Inquiry Meetings 1931-1938": Correction to Deary, Lawn, and Bartholomew (2008)" (PDF). History of Psychology. 11 (3): 156–157. doi:10.1037/1093-4510.11.3.163. hdl:20.500.11820/5417f3c7-e873-40b9-ad73-19c6acc9e35b. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2020-08-06. Retrieved 2020-06-25.
- Jensen 1998, p. 445ff.
- Ceci, Stephen J. (1991). "How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence". Developmental Psychology. 27 (5): 703–722. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.27.5.703.
- ^ Dickens & Flynn 2006.
- Richie, Stuart J.; Tucker-Drob, Elliot (June 2018). "How Much Does Education Improve Intelligence? A Meta-Analysis". Psychological Science. 29 (8).
- Mackintosh 2011, p. 359.
- ^ Using Population Descriptors in Genetics and Genomics Research: A New Framework for an Evolving Field (Consensus Study Report). National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2023. doi:10.17226/26902. ISBN 978-0-309-70065-8. PMID 36989389.
In humans, race is a socially constructed designation, a misleading and harmful surrogate for population genetic differences, and has a long history of being incorrectly identified as the major genetic reason for phenotypic differences between groups.
- Daley & Onwuegbuzie 2011, p. 294.
- Templeton, A. (2016). EVOLUTION AND NOTIONS OF HUMAN RACE. In Losos J. & Lenski R. (Eds.), How Evolution Shapes Our Lives: Essays on Biology and Society (pp. 346–361). Princeton; Oxford: Princeton University Press. doi:10.2307/j.ctv7h0s6j.26. That this view reflects the consensus among American anthropologists is stated in: Wagner, Jennifer K.; Yu, Joon-Ho; Ifekwunigwe, Jayne O.; Harrell, Tanya M.; Bamshad, Michael J.; Royal, Charmaine D. (February 2017). "Anthropologists' views on race, ancestry, and genetics". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 162 (2): 318–327. doi:10.1002/ajpa.23120. PMC 5299519. PMID 27874171. See also: American Association of Physical Anthropologists (27 March 2019). "AAPA Statement on Race and Racism". American Association of Physical Anthropologists. Archived from the original on 25 January 2022. Retrieved 19 June 2020.
- Smay & Armelagos 2000.
- Rotimi, Charles N. (2004). "Are medical and nonmedical uses of large-scale genomic markers conflating genetics and 'race'?". Nature Genetics. 36 (11 Suppl): 43–47. doi:10.1038/ng1439. PMID 15508002.
Two facts are relevant: (i) as a result of different evolutionary forces, including natural selection, there are geographical patterns of genetic variations that correspond, for the most part, to continental origin; and (ii) observed patterns of geographical differences in genetic information do not correspond to our notion of social identities, including 'race' and 'ethnicity
- Schaefer 2008
- AAA 1998
- "AAPA Statement on Race & Racism". Archived from the original on 2022-01-25. Retrieved 2020-06-28.
- Brace 2005
- Kaplan, Jonathan Michael; Winther, Rasmus Grønfeldt (2014). "Realism, Antirealism, and Conventionalism About Race". Philosophy of Science. 81 (5): 1039–1052. doi:10.1086/678314. S2CID 55148854.
- Winther, Rasmus Grønfeldt (2015). "The Genetic Reification of 'Race'?: A Story of Two Mathematical Methods" (PDF). Critical Philosophy of Race. 2 (2): 204–223.
- Kaplan & Winther (2013).
- Graves, Joseph (7 June 2006). "What We Know and What We Don't Know: Human Genetic Variation and the Social Construction of Race". Race and Genomics. Archived from the original on 3 June 2019. Retrieved 3 December 2023.
- Weiss, K. M.; Fullerton, S. M. (2005). "Racing around, getting nowhere". Evolutionary Anthropology. 14 (5): 165–169. doi:10.1002/evan.20079. S2CID 84927946.
- Mills, C. W. (1988). "But What Are You Really? The Metaphysics of Race". Blackness visible: essays on philosophy and race. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press. pp. 41–66.
- Rowe 2005
- ^ Hunt & Carlson 2007
- Sternberg & Grigorenko 2007
- ^ Neisser et al. 1996.
- ^ Jackson & Weidman 2004, p. 222.
- Hampshire et al. 2012.
- Reynolds, Cecil R.; Altmann, Robert A.; Allen, Daniel N. (2021). "The Problem of Bias in Psychological Assessment". Mastering Modern Psychological Testing. Springer. pp. 573–613, 582. doi:10.1007/978-3-030-59455-8_15. ISBN 978-3-030-59454-1. S2CID 236660997.
- Encyclopedia of Diversity in Education. SAGE. 2012. p. 1209. ISBN 978-1-4129-8152-1. Archived from the original on 2023-03-20. Retrieved 2018-01-20.
- Roth et al. 2001
- Demie, Feyisa; McLean, Christabel (1 December 2007). "Raising the achievement of African heritage pupils: a case study of good practice in British schools". Educational Studies. 33 (4): 415–434. doi:10.1080/03055690701423606. ISSN 0305-5698. S2CID 144579288.
- Rutter, Jill (2013). Back to basics: Towards a successful and cost-effective integration policy. Institute for Public Policy Research. p. 43. Archived from the original on 2020-04-13. Retrieved 2020-05-23.
{{cite book}}
:|work=
ignored (help) - Feyisa Demie, Andrew Hau (2016). Language Diversity and Attainment in Primary Schools in England (PDF). Lambeth Research And Statistics Unit. p. 18. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2020-08-06. Retrieved 2020-05-24.
- "GCSE English and maths results". Gov.UK. 2019. Archived from the original on 2022-09-20. Retrieved 2022-09-20.
- Mackintosh 1998, p. 162
- Swain, Carol (2003). Contemporary voices of white nationalism in America. Cambridge, UK New York: Cambridge University Press. p. 70. ISBN 978-0-521-01693-3. Note: this quote is from the authors' introductory essay, not from the interviews.
- Dickens & Flynn 2001.
- Vincent 1991.
- Neisser, Ulric (Ed). 1998. The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association
- Mackintosh 2011.
- ^ Nisbett et al. 2012a.
- ^ Bellinger, Stiles & Needleman 1992
- MMWR 2005
- ^ Campbell et al. 2002
- Ivanovic et al. 2004
- Saloojee & Pettifor 2001
- Agency For Toxic Substances And Disease Registry Case Studies In Environmental Medicine (CSEM) (2012-02-15). "Principles of Pediatric Environmental Health, The Child as Susceptible Host: A Developmental Approach to Pediatric Environmental Medicine" (PDF). U.S. Department for Health and Human Services. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2019-01-31. Retrieved 2019-01-30.
- Lanphear, Bruce P.; Hornung, Richard; Khoury, Jane; Yolton, Kimberly; Baghurst, Peter; Bellinger, David C.; Canfield, Richard L.; Dietrich, Kim N.; Bornschein, Robert; Greene, Tom; Rothenberg, Stephen J.; Needleman, Herbert L.; Schnaas, Lourdes; Wasserman, Gail; Graziano, Joseph; Roberts, Russell (2005-03-18). "Low-Level Environmental Lead Exposure and Children's Intellectual Function: An International Pooled Analysis". Environmental Health Perspectives. 113 (7): 894–899. Bibcode:2005EnvHP.113..894L. doi:10.1289/ehp.7688. ISSN 0091-6765. PMC 1257652. PMID 16002379.
- Qian et al. 2005
- Feyrer, James; Politi, Dimitra; Weil, David N. (2017). "The Cognitive Effects of Micronutrient Deficiency: Evidence from Salt Iodization in the United States" (PDF). Journal of the European Economic Association. 15 (2): 355–387. doi:10.1093/jeea/jvw002. PMC 6919660. PMID 31853231. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2020-08-13. Retrieved 2019-07-22.
- The Lancet Series on Maternal and Child Undernutrition Archived 2011-07-17 at the Wayback Machine, 2008.
- Mackintosh 2011, pp. 343–44.
- Behrman, Alderman & Hoddinott 2004
- Colom, R.; Lluis-Font, J. M.; Andrés-Pueyo, A. (2005). "The generational intelligence gains are caused by decreasing variance in the lower half of the distribution: supporting evidence for the nutrition hypothesis". Intelligence. 33: 83–91. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.010.
- Flynn, J. R. (2009a). "Requiem for nutrition as the cause of IQ gains: Raven's gains in Britain 1938 to 2008". Economics and Human Biology. 7 (1): 18–27. doi:10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.009. PMID 19251490.
- Eppig 2011
- Horta, Bernardo L.; Victoria, Cesar G. (2013). "Long-term effects of breastfeeding – a systemic review" (PDF). World Health Organization. Archived (PDF) from the original on 9 April 2020. Retrieved 18 June 2018.
- Colen, Cynthia G.; Ramey, David M. (2014). "Is Breast Truly Best? Estimating the Effect of Breastfeeding on Long-term Child Wellbeing in the United States Using Sibling Comparisons". Social Science & Medicine. 109 (1): 55–65. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.01.027. PMC 4077166. PMID 24698713.
- Reichman 2005
- Manly et al. 2002 and Manly et al. 2004
- Mickelson 2003
- Elhoweris et al. 2005
- Miele 2002, p. 133
- Fagan, Joseph F; Holland, Cynthia R (2002). "Equal opportunity and racial differences in IQ". Intelligence. 30 (4): 361–387. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00080-6.
- Fagan, J.F.; Holland, C.R. (2007). "Racial equality in intelligence: Predictions from a theory of intelligence as processing". Intelligence. 35 (4): 319–334. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.08.009.
- Daley & Onwuegbuzie 2011.
- Marks, D.F. (2010). "IQ variations across time, race, and nationality: An artifact of differences in literacy skills". Psychological Reports. 106 (3): 643–664. doi:10.2466/pr0.106.3.643-664. PMID 20712152. S2CID 12179547.
- Barry, Scott (2010-08-23). "The Flynn Effect and IQ Disparities Among Races, Ethnicities, and Nations: Are There Common Links?". Psychology Today. Archived from the original on 2023-03-20. Retrieved 2014-08-22.
- McKay, Patrick F.; Doverspike, Dennis; Bowen-Hilton, Doreen; McKay, Quintonia D. (2003). "The Effects of Demographic Variables and Stereotype Threat on Black/White Differences in Cognitive Ability Test Performance". Journal of Business and Psychology. 18 (1): 1–14. doi:10.1023/A:1025062703113. S2CID 142317051.
- Hunt 2010, p. 428.
- Magnuson, Katherine A.; Duncan, Greg J. (December 2006). "The role of family socioeconomic resources in the black–white test score gap among young children". Developmental Review. 26 (4): 365–399. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2006.06.004.
- Scarr-Salapatek, S. (1971). "Race, social class, and IQ". Science. 174 (4016): 1285–95. Bibcode:1971Sci...174.1285S. doi:10.1126/science.174.4016.1285. PMID 5167501.
- Scarr-Salapatek, S. (1974). "Some myths about heritability and IQ". Nature. 251 (5475): 463–464. Bibcode:1974Natur.251..463S. doi:10.1038/251463b0. S2CID 32437709.
- D. C. Rowe. (1994). The Limits of Family Influence: Genes, Experience and Behaviour. Guilford Press. London
- Kirkpatrick, R. M.; McGue, M.; Iacono, W. G. (2015). "Replication of a gene-environment interaction Via Multimodel inference: additive-genetic variance in adolescents' general cognitive ability increases with family-of-origin socioeconomic status". Behav Genet. 45 (2): 200–14. doi:10.1007/s10519-014-9698-y. PMC 4374354. PMID 25539975.
- Bronfenbrenner, Urie; Ceci, Stephen J. (October 1994). "Nature-nuture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model". Psychological Review. 101 (4): 568–586. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.101.4.568. PMID 7984707. S2CID 17402964.
- Cottrell, Newman & Roisman 2015.
- Cronshaw et al. 2006, p. 278
- Verney et al. 2005
- Borsboom 2006
- Shuttleworth-Edwards et al. 2004
- Richardson 2004
- Hunt & Wittmann 2008
- Irvine 1983
- Irvine & Berry 1988 a collection of articles by several authors discussing the limits of assessment by intelligence tests in different communities in the world. In particular, Reuning (1988) describes the difficulties in devising and administering tests for Kalahari bushmen.
- Mackintosh 1998, p. 174: "Despite widespread belief to the contrary, however, there is ample evidence, both in Britain and the USA, that IQ tests predict educational attainment just about as well in ethnic minorities as in the white majority."
- "Reflections about intelligence over 40 years" (PDF). Intelligence (70): 73–83. 2018. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2019-02-03. Retrieved 2019-02-02.
- Aronson, Wilson & Akert 2005
- Steele, Claude M. (1997). "A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance". American Psychologist. 52 (6): 613–629. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.319.8283. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 9174398. S2CID 19952.
- Mackintosh 2011, p. 348.
- Ogbu 1978.
- Ogbu 1994.
- Chan, D.; Schmitt, N.; DeShon, R. P.; Clause, C. S.; Delbridge, K. (April 1997). "Reactions to cognitive ability tests: the relationships between race, test performance, face validity perceptions, and test-taking motivation". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 82 (2): 300–310. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.82.2.300. ISSN 0021-9010. PMID 9109288.
- Ganley CM, Mingle LA, Ryan AM, Ryan K, Vasilyeva M, Perry M (October 2013). "An examination of stereotype threat effects on girls' mathematics performance" (PDF). Developmental Psychology. 49 (10): 1886–97. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.353.4436. doi:10.1037/a0031412. PMID 23356523. Archived from the original (PDF) on 19 July 2014.
- Stoet G, Geary DC (2012). "Can stereotype threat explain the gender gap in mathematics performance and achievement?". Review of General Psychology. 16: 93–102. doi:10.1037/a0026617. S2CID 145724069. Pdf. Archived 2016-01-12 at the Wayback Machine
- Flore PC, Wicherts JM (February 2015). "Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis". Journal of School Psychology. 53 (1): 25–44. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002. PMID 25636259. S2CID 206516995.
- Sackett PR, Hardison CM, Cullen MJ (January 2004). "On interpreting stereotype threat as accounting for African American-White differences on cognitive tests" (PDF). The American Psychologist. 59 (1): 7–13. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.7. PMID 14736315. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-04-04.
- Pennington CR, Heim D, Levy AR, Larkin DT (2016-01-11). "Twenty Years of Stereotype Threat Research: A Review of Psychological Mediators". PLOS ONE. 11 (1): e0146487. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1146487P. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0146487. PMC 4713435. PMID 26752551.
- ^ Nguyen HH, Ryan AM (November 2008). "Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 93 (6): 1314–34. doi:10.1037/a0012702. PMID 19025250. S2CID 36769821.
- ^ Walton, Gregory M.; Spencer, Steven J. (2009-09-01). "Latent Ability: Grades and Test Scores Systematically Underestimate the Intellectual Ability of Negatively Stereotyped Students". Psychological Science. 20 (9): 1132–1139. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02417.x. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 19656335. S2CID 25810191.
- Gentile, Ambra; Boca, Stefano; Giammusso, Isabella (2018-11-01). "'You play like a Woman!' Effects of gender stereotype threat on Women's performance in physical and sport activities: A meta-analysis". Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 39: 95–103. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.07.013. ISSN 1469-0292. S2CID 149490634.
- Lamont, Ruth A.; Swift, Hannah J.; Abrams, Dominic (2015). "A Review and Meta-Analysis of Age-Based Stereotype Threat: Negative Stereotypes, Not Facts, Do the Damage". Psychology and Aging. 30 (1): 180–193. doi:10.1037/a0038586. ISSN 1939-1498. PMC 4360754. PMID 25621742.
- Picho, Katherine; Rodriguez, Ariel; Finnie, Lauren (May 2013). "Exploring the Moderating Role of Context on the Mathematics Performance of Females Under Stereotype Threat: A Meta-Analysis". The Journal of Social Psychology. 153 (3): 299–333. doi:10.1080/00224545.2012.737380. PMID 23724702. S2CID 45950675.
- Liu, Songqi; Liu, Pei; Wang, Mo; Zhang, Baoshan (July 2020). "Effectiveness of Stereotype Threat Interventions: A Meta-Analytic Review". Journal of Applied Psychology. currently in press (6): 921–949. doi:10.1037/apl0000770. PMID 32772526. S2CID 221098319.
- Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (2012). "Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments". American Psychologist. 67 (2): 130–159. doi:10.1037/a0026699. ISSN 1935-990X. PMID 22233090.
- ^ Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (2012). "Group differences in IQ are best understood as environmental in origin". American Psychologist. 67 (6): 503–504. doi:10.1037/a0029772. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 22963427.
- Bird, Kevin; Jackson, John P.; Winston, Andrew S. (2024). "Confronting Scientific Racism in Psychology: Lessons from Evolutionary Biology and Genetics". American Psychologist. 79 (4): 497–508. doi:10.1037/amp0001228. PMID 39037836.
Recent articles claim that the folk categories of race are genetically meaningful divisions, and that evolved genetic differences among races and nations are important for explaining immutable differences in cognitive ability, educational attainment, crime, sexual behavior, and wealth; all claims that are opposed by a strong scientific consensus to the contrary.
- Ceci & Williams 2009, pp. 788–789, "There is an emerging consensus about racial and gender equality in genetic determinants of intelligence, most researchers, including ourselves, agree that genes do not explain between-group differences".
- Hunt 2010, p. 447, "It is worth remembering that no genes related to difference in cognitive skills across the various racial and ethnic groups have ever been discovered. The argument for genetic differences has been carried forward largely by circumstantial evidence. Of course, tomorrow afternoon genetic mechanisms producing racial and ethnic differences in intelligence might be discovered, but there have been a lot of investigations, and tomorrow has not come for quite some time now."
- Mackintosh 2011, pp. 334–338, 344.
- ^ Kaplan, Jonathan Michael (January 2015). "Race, IQ, and the search for statistical signals associated with so-called "X"-factors: environments, racism, and the "hereditarian hypothesis"". Biology & Philosophy. 30 (1): 1–17. doi:10.1007/s10539-014-9428-0. ISSN 0169-3867. S2CID 85351431.
- Panofsky, Aaron; Dasgupta, Kushan; Iturriaga, Nicole (2021). "How White nationalists mobilize genetics: From genetic ancestry and human biodiversity to counterscience and metapolitics". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 175 (2): 387–398. doi:10.1002/ajpa.24150. ISSN 0002-9483. PMC 9909835. PMID 32986847.
he claims that genetics defines racial groups and makes them different, that IQ and cultural differences among racial groups are caused by genes, and that racial inequalities within and between nations are the inevitable outcome of long evolutionary processes are neither new nor supported by science (either old or new).
- Lewontin, Richard C. (March 1970). "Race and Intelligence". Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists. 26 (3): 2–8. Bibcode:1970BuAtS..26c...2L. doi:10.1080/00963402.1970.11457774. Archived from the original on 10 June 2021. Retrieved 26 April 2021.
- Nevid 2014, p. 271.
- ^ Templeton 2001
- ^ "Intelligence research should not be held back by its past". Nature. 545 (7655): 385–386. 25 May 2017. Bibcode:2017Natur.545R.385.. doi:10.1038/nature.2017.22021. PMID 28541341. S2CID 4449918.
- Bird, Kevin A. (2 February 2021). "No support for the hereditarian hypothesis of the Black–White achievement gap using polygenic scores and tests for divergent selection". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 175 (2): 465–476. doi:10.1002/ajpa.24216. ISSN 0002-9483. PMID 33529393. Retrieved 1 November 2024 – via Wiley Online Library.
- Carson, Michael; Beckwith, Jon (2001). 'Race', IQ and Genes. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. pp. 1–5. doi:10.1002/9780470015902.a0005689.pub3. ISBN 978-0-470-01590-2.
- Beckwith, Jon; Morris, Corey A. (December 2008). "Twin Studies of Political Behavior: Untenable Assumptions?". Perspectives on Politics. 6 (4): 785–791. doi:10.1017/S1537592708081917. ISSN 1541-0986. S2CID 55630117.
- Kamin, Leon J.; Goldberger, Arthur S. (February 2002). "Twin Studies in Behavioral Research: A Skeptical View". Theoretical Population Biology. 61 (1): 83–95. Bibcode:2002TPBio..61...83K. doi:10.1006/tpbi.2001.1555. ISSN 0040-5809. PMID 11895384.
- Moore & Shenk 2016.
- Mackenzie 1984
- Deary, Johnson & Houlihan 2009
- ^ Block 2002
- Rushton & Jensen 2005.
- Flynn, J.R. (1999). "Searching for justice: the discovery of IQ gains over time" (PDF). American Psychologist. 54: 5–9. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.1.5. Archived (PDF) from the original on 25 June 2010. Retrieved 26 October 2017.
- Flynn, James R. (2010). "The spectacles through which I see the race and IQ debate" (PDF). Intelligence. 38 (4): 363–366. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.05.001. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2020-12-07. Retrieved 2011-02-18.
- Flynn, James R.; te Nijenhuis, Jan; Metzen, Daniel (2014). "The g beyond Spearman's g: Flynn's paradoxes resolved using four exploratory meta-analyses" (PDF). Intelligence. 44: 1–10. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.01.009.
- ^ Mackintosh 2011, p. 337.
- ^ Weinberg, Scarr & Waldman 1992
- Scarr & Weinberg 1976.
- Loehlin 2000, p. 185.
- Scarr & Weinberg 1990.
- Loehlin 2000, p. 187.
- Frydman and Lynn (1989). "The intelligence of Korean children adopted in Belgium". Personality and Individual Differences. 10 (12): 1323–1325. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(89)90246-8.
- Thomas, Drew (2017). "Racial IQ Differences among Transracial Adoptees: Fact or Artifact?". Journal of Intelligence. 5 (1): 1. doi:10.3390/jintelligence5010001. PMC 6526420. PMID 31162392.
- Bryc et al. 2009
- Loehlin 2000.
- Templeton 2001.
- Sheppard & Vernon 2008.
- ^ Jensen 2006
- Nisbett 2009
- Nisbett 2009, pp. 221–2.
- Deary, Penke & Johnson 2010.
- McDaniel 2005.
- Ho et al. 1980.
- MacEachern 2006.
- Gray & Thompson 2004
- Ceci & Williams 2009
- ^ Rose, Steven (2009). "Should scientists study race and IQ? NO: Science and society do not benefit". Nature. 457 (7231): 786–788. Bibcode:2009Natur.457..786R. doi:10.1038/457786a. PMID 19212384. S2CID 42846614.
- Flynn 2009b
- Jencks, Christopher; Phillips, Meredith. "The Black-White Test Score Gap". New York Times. Archived from the original on 8 October 2016. Retrieved 2 October 2016.
- Flynn 2008
- Olness 2003
Bibliography
- Alland, Alexander Jr (2002). Race in Mind. Palgrave Macmillan. pp. 79–80. ISBN 978-0-312-23838-4.
- "American Anthropological Association Statement on 'Race' and Intelligence". American Anthropological Association. December 1994. Archived from the original on 2005-03-05. Retrieved 2005-03-24.
- "American Anthropological Association Statement on 'Race'". American Anthropological Association. 17 May 1998. Archived from the original on 27 June 2013. Retrieved 24 February 2011.
- "AAPA Statement on Biological Aspects of Race". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 101 (4): 569–570. 1996. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1331010408. Archived from the original on 2011-09-28. Retrieved 2011-05-24.
- Aronson, E; Wilson, TD; Akert, AM (2005). Social Psychology (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. ISBN 978-0-13-178686-8.
- Behrman, JR; Alderman, H; Hoddinott, J (2004). "Hunger and Malnutrition" (PDF). Copenhagen Consensus. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2006-10-25.
- Bellinger, David C; Stiles, Karen M; Needleman, Herbert L (December 1992). "Low-Level Lead Exposure, Intelligence and Academic Achievement: A Long-term Follow-up Study". Pediatrics. 90 (6): 855–61. doi:10.1542/peds.90.6.855. PMID 1437425. S2CID 25746146. Archived from the original on 2019-01-04. Retrieved 2009-12-19.
- Berlet, Chip (Summer 2003). "Into the Mainstream". Intelligence Report (110). Archived from the original on 2 February 2010. Retrieved 21 February 2012.
- Block, Ned (2002). "How heritability misleads about race". In Fish, Jefferson (ed.). Race and Intelligence: Separating Science from Myth. Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.
- Borsboom, Denny (September 2006). "The attack of the psychometricians". Psychometrika. 71 (3): 425–40. doi:10.1007/s11336-006-1447-6. PMC 2779444. PMID 19946599.
- Brace, C Loring (1999). "An Anthropological Perspective on 'Race' and Intelligence: The non-clinal nature of human cognitive capabilities". Journal of Anthropological Research. 55 (2): 245–64. doi:10.1086/jar.55.2.3631210. JSTOR 3631210. S2CID 83112950.
- Brace, C. Loring (2005). Race is a four letter word. Oxford University Press. p. 326. ISBN 978-0-19-517351-2.
- "race". Encyclopædia Britannica (Online ed.). Encyclopædia Britannica Inc. 2012. Archived from the original on 2011-06-29. Retrieved 2011-02-24.
- Brown, Robert T; Reynolds, Cecil R; Whitaker, Jean S (1999). "Bias in Mental Testing since "Bias in Mental Testing"". School Psychology Quarterly. 14 (3): 208–38. doi:10.1037/h0089007. S2CID 46561407.
- Bryc, Katarzyna; Auton, Adam; Nelson, Matthew R.; Oksenberg, Jorge R.; Hauser, Stephen L.; Williams, Scott; Froment, Alain; Bodo, Jean-Marie; Wambebe, Charles; Tishkoff, Sarah A.; Bustamante, Carlos D. (2009). "Genome-wide patterns of population structure and admixture in West Africans and African Americans". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (2): 786–91. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107..786B. doi:10.1073/pnas.0909559107. PMC 2818934. PMID 20080753.
- Campbell, FA; Ramey, CT (1994). "Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic achievement: A follow-up study of children from low-income families". Child Development. 65 (2): 684–698. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00777.x.
- Campbell, Frances A; Ramey, Craig T; Pungello, Elizabeth; Sparling, Joseph; Miller-Johnson, Shari (2002). "Early Childhood Education: Young Adult Outcomes From the Abecedarian Project". Applied Developmental Science. 6: 42–57. doi:10.1207/s1532480xads0601_05. S2CID 71602425.
- Carson, Michael; Beckwith, Jon (2001). 'Race', IQ and Genes. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. pp. 1–5. doi:10.1002/9780470015902.a0005689.pub3. ISBN 978-0-470-01590-2.
- Ceci, SJ; Williams, WM (2009). "Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? Yes: the scientific truth must be pursued". Nature. 457 (7231): 788–9. Bibcode:2009Natur.457..788C. doi:10.1038/457788a. PMID 19212385. S2CID 205044224.
- Cooper, R. S. (2005). "Race and IQ: Molecular Genetics as Deus ex Machina". American Psychologist. 60 (1): 71–76. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.624.5059. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.71. PMID 15641923.
- Cottrell, Jonathan M.; Newman, Daniel A.; Roisman, Glenn I. (November 2015). "Explaining the black-white gap in cognitive test scores: Toward a theory of adverse impact". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 100 (6): 1713–1736. doi:10.1037/apl0000020. ISSN 1939-1854. PMID 25867168.
- Cronshaw, Steven F; Hamilton, Leah K; Onyura, Betty R; Winston, Andrew S (September 2006). "Case for Non-Biased Intelligence Testing Against Black Africans Has Not Been Made: A Comment on Rushton, Skuy, and Bons". International Journal of Selection and Assessment. 14 (3): 278–287. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2389.2006.00346.x. S2CID 91179275.
- Daley, C. E.; Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2011). "Race and Intelligence". In Sternberg, R.; Kaufman, S. B. (eds.). The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 293–306. ISBN 978-0-521-51806-2.
- Deary, Ian J (2001). Intelligence: A Very Short Introduction. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-289321-5.
- Deary, I. J.; Penke, L.; Johnson, W. (2010). "The neuroscience of human intelligence differences" (PDF). Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 11 (3): 201–211. doi:10.1038/nrn2793. hdl:20.500.11820/9b11fac3-47d0-424c-9d1c-fe6f9ff2ecac. PMID 20145623. S2CID 5136934. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2018-07-19. Retrieved 2019-01-26.
- Deary, IJ; Johnson, W; Houlihan, LM (2009). "Genetic foundations of human intelligence" (PDF). Human Genetics. 126 (1): 215–32. doi:10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4. hdl:20.500.11820/c3e0a75b-dad6-4860-91c6-b242221af681. PMID 19294424. S2CID 4975607. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2020-08-01. Retrieved 2019-12-07.
- Dickens, William T; Flynn, James R (2001). "Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved". Psychological Review. 108 (2): 346–69. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.139.2436. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346. PMID 11381833.
- Dickens, William T; Flynn, James R (2006). "Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence from Standardization Samples" (PDF). Psychological Science. 17 (10): 913–20. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.186.2540. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01802.x. PMID 17100793. S2CID 6593169. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2009-10-09.
- Elhoweris, Hala; Mutua, Kagendo; Alsheikh, Negmeldin; Holloway, Pauline (2005). "Effect of Children's Ethnicity on Teachers' Referral and Recommendation Decisions in Gifted and Talented Programs". Remedial and Special Education. 26: 25–31. doi:10.1177/07419325050260010401. S2CID 146321193.
- Eppig, Christopher (2011). "Why Is Average IQ Higher in Some Places?". Scientific American. Archived from the original on 2020-04-01. Retrieved 2011-11-20.
- Evans, Gavin (2019). Skin Deep: Journeys in the Divisive Science of Race. London: Oneworld Publications. ISBN 978-1-78607-622-9. OCLC 1059232398.
- Eyferth, K (1961). "Leistungern verscheidener Gruppen von Besatzungskindern Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest für Kinder (HAWIK)". Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie (in German). 113: 222–41.
- Eysenck, Hans J (1987). "Intelligence and Reaction Time: The Contribution of Arthur Jensen". In Modgil, S; Modgil, C (eds.). Arthur Jensen: Consensus and controversy. New York, NY: Falmer.
- Falk, Avner (2008). Anti-semitism: a history and psychoanalysis of contemporary hatred. Praeger. p. 312. ISBN 978-0-313-35385-7.
- Flynn, James R (1991). "Reaction times show that both Chinese and British children are more intelligent than one another". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 72 (2): 544–6. doi:10.2466/pms.1991.72.2.544. S2CID 144091744.
- Flynn, James R (3 September 2008). "Perspectives: Still a question of black vs white?". New Scientist. 199 (2672) (magazine issue ed.): 48–50. doi:10.1016/S0262-4079(08)62253-8. Archived from the original on 7 July 2011. Retrieved 6 February 2011.
- Flynn, James R (2009b). "Would you wish the research undone?". Nature. 458 (7235): 146. Bibcode:2009Natur.458..146F. doi:10.1038/458146a. PMID 19279612.
- Flynn, James R (2010). "The spectacles through which I see the race and IQ debate" (PDF). Intelligence. 38 (4): 363–6. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.05.001. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2020-12-07. Retrieved 2011-02-18.
- Flynn, James R. (2012). Are We Getting Smarter? Rising IQ in the Twenty-First Century. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-1-107-60917-4.
- Lay summary in: Lea Winerman (March 2013). "Smarter than ever?". Monitor on Psychology.
- Gottfredson, Linda S (1997). "Mainstream Science on Intelligence (editorial)" (PDF). Intelligence. 24: 13–23. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90011-8. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2014-12-22. Retrieved 2005-01-08.
- Gottfredson, Linda S (2007). "Applying Double Standards to 'Divisive' Ideas: Commentary on Hunt and Carlson". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2 (2): 216–220. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00039.x. PMID 26151962. S2CID 21203607.
- Gould, Stephen Jay (1981). The Mismeasure of Man. New York, London: Norton. ISBN 978-0-393-30056-7.
- Graves, Joseph L. (2001). The Emperor's New Clothes: Biological Theories of Race at the Millennium (Kindle ed.). Rutgers University Press. ISBN 978-0-8135-2847-2.
- Graves, Joseph L (2002a). "The Misuse of Life History Theory: JP Rushton and the Pseudoscience of Racial Hierarchy". In Fish, Jefferson M (ed.). Race and Intelligence: Separating Myth from Reality. Laurence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 57–94. ISBN 978-0-8058-3757-5.
- Graves, Joseph L Jr (2002b). "What a tangled web he weaves: Race, reproductive strategies and Rushton's life history theory". Anthropological Theory. 2 (2): 131–54. doi:10.1177/1469962002002002627. S2CID 144377864.
- Gray, Jeremy R; Thompson, Paul M (2004). "Neurobiology of intelligence: science and ethics" (PDF). Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 5 (6): 471–82. doi:10.1038/nrn1405. PMID 15152197. S2CID 2430677. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2018-07-14. Retrieved 2019-05-10.
- Grossman, James B; Kaufman (2001). "Evolutionary Psychology: Promise and Perils". In Sternberg, Robert J; Kaufman, James C (eds.). The evolution of intelligence. Routledge. ISBN 978-0-8058-3267-9.
- Hampshire, A.; Highfield, R. R.; Parkin, B. L.; Owen, A. M. (2012). "Fractionating human intelligence". Neuron. 76 (6): 1225–1237. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.022. PMID 23259956.
- Herrnstein, Richard J; Murray, Charles (1994). The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life. New York: Free Press. ISBN 978-0-02-914673-6.
- Ho, KC; Roessmann, U; Straumfjord, JV; Monroe, G (December 1980). "Analysis of brain weight. I. Adult brain weight in relation to sex, race, and age". Archives of Pathology & Laboratory Medicine. 104 (12): 635–9. PMID 6893659.
- Hunt, Earl (2010). Human Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-70781-7.
- Hunt, Earl; Carlson, Jerry (2007). "Considerations relating to the study of group differences in intelligence". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2 (2): 194–213. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00037.x. PMID 26151960. S2CID 32540018.
- Hunt, Earl; Wittmann, Werner (January–February 2008). "National intelligence and national prosperity". Intelligence. 36 (1): 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.11.002.
- Irvine, SH (1983). "Where intelligence tests fail". Nature. 302 (5907): 371. Bibcode:1983Natur.302..371I. doi:10.1038/302371b0. S2CID 41604729.
- Irvine, SH; Berry, JW, eds. (1988). Human Abilities in Cultural Context. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-34482-1.
- Ivanovic, Daniza M.; Leiva, Boris P.; Pérez, Hernán T.; Olivares, Manuel G.; Dı́az, Nora S.; Urruti, Marı́a Soledad C.; Almagià, Atilio F.; Toro, Triana D.; Miller, Patricio T.; Bosch, Enrique O.; Larraı́n, Cristián G. (2004). "Head size and intelligence, learning, nutritional status and brain development. Head, IQ, learning, nutrition and brain". Neuropsychologia. 42 (8): 1118–31. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.022. PMID 15093150. S2CID 2114185.
- Jackson, John P.; Weidman, Nadine M. (2004). Race, Racism, and Science: Social Impact and Interaction. ABC-CLIO. p. 23.
- Jackson, John P. (2005). Science for Segregation: Race, Law, and the Case against Brown v. Board of Education. NYU Press. ISBN 978-0-8147-4271-6.
- Lay summary in: "Book Review: Science for Segregation: Race, Law, and the Case Against Brown v. Board of Education". History Cooperative.
- Jensen, Arthur R (1969). "How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement?". Harvard Educational Review. 39: 1–123. doi:10.17763/haer.39.1.l3u15956627424k7.
- Jensen, Arthur R (1973). Educability and Group Differences. London: Methuen. ISBN 978-0-06-012194-5.
- Jensen, Arthur R; Whang, PA (1993). "Reaction times and intelligence: a comparison of Chinese-American and Anglo-American children". Journal of Biosocial Science. 25 (3): 397–410. doi:10.1017/s0021932000020721. PMID 8360233. S2CID 18171436.
- Jensen, Arthur R (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport, CT: Praeger. ISBN 978-0-275-96103-9.
- Jensen, AR (2006). Clocking the mind: Mental chronometry and individual differences. Amsterdam: Elsevier. ISBN 978-0-08-044939-5.
- Jensen, Arthur R; Johnson, Fred W (May–June 1994). "Race and sex differences in head size and IQ". Intelligence. 18 (3): 309–33. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(94)90032-9.
- Kamin, Leon J. (March 2006). "African IQ and Mental Retardation". South African Journal of Psychology. 36 (1): 1–9. doi:10.1177/008124630603600101. S2CID 92984213.
- Kaplan, Jonathan Michael (January 2011). "'Race': What Biology Can Tell Us about a Social Construct". Encyclopedia of Life Sciences. doi:10.1002/9780470015902.a0005857. ISBN 978-0-470-01617-6. Archived from the original on 2021-05-25. Retrieved 2012-02-22.
- Kaplan, Jonathan Michael (6 February 2014). "Race, IQ, and the search for statistical signals associated with so-called "X"-factors: environments, racism, and the "hereditarian hypothesis"". Biology & Philosophy. 30 (1): 1–17. doi:10.1007/s10539-014-9428-0. S2CID 85351431.
- Kaplan, J. M.; Winther, R. G. (2013). "Prisoners of Abstraction? The Theory and Measure of Genetic Variation, and the Very Concept of 'Race'" (PDF). Biological Theory. 7: 401–412. doi:10.1002/9780470015902.a0005857.
- Kaszycka, Katarzyna A.; Štrkalj, Goran; Strzałko, Jan (2009). "Current Views of European Anthropologists on Race: Influence of Educational and Ideological Background". American Anthropologist. 111 (1): 43–56. doi:10.1111/j.1548-1433.2009.01076.x.
- Lieberman, Leonard (2001). "How "Caucasoids" got such big crania and why they shrank: from Morton to Rushton."". Current Anthropology. 42 (1): 69–95. doi:10.1086/318434. PMID 14992214. S2CID 224794908.
- Loehlin, John C (2000). "Group Differences in Intelligence". In Sternberg, Robert J (ed.). The Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Ludy, Benjamin T (2006). Brief History of Modern Psychology. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 188–91. ISBN 978-1-4051-3206-0.
- MacEachern, Scott (March 2006). "Africanist archaeology and ancient IQ: racial science and cultural evolution in the twenty-first century". World Archaeology. 38 (1): 72–92. doi:10.1080/00438240500509918. S2CID 162340124.
- Mackenzie, Brian (1984). "Explaining race differences in IQ: The logic, the methodology, and the evidence". American Psychologist. 39 (11): 1214–1233. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.39.11.1214.
- Mackintosh, N. J. (1998). IQ and Human Intelligence. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-852367-3.
- Mackintosh, N. J. (2011). IQ and Human Intelligence (second ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-958559-5.
- Manly, JJ; Byrd, DA; Touradji, P; Stern, Yaakov (2004). "Acculturation, Reading Level, and Neuropsychological Test Performance Among African American Elders". Applied Neuropsychology. 11 (1): 37–46. doi:10.1207/s15324826an1101_5. PMID 15471745. S2CID 30490056.
- Manly, Jennifer J; Jacobs, Diane M; Touradji, Pegah; Small, Scott A; Stern, Yaakov (2002). "Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders". Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 8 (3): 341–348. doi:10.1017/S1355617702813157. PMID 11939693. S2CID 14360177.
- Maltby, John; Day, Liz; Macaskill, Ann (2007). Personality, Individual Differences and Intelligence. Pearson Education. pp. 334–47. ISBN 978-0-13-129760-9.
- McDaniel, M. A. (2005). "Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence". Intelligence. 33 (4): 337–346. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.11.005.
- Mickelson, Roslyn Arlin (2003). "When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? A Social Science Perspective". Teachers College Record. 105 (6): 1052–1086. doi:10.1111/1467-9620.00277.
- Miele, Frank (2002). Intelligence, Race and Genetics: Conversations with Arthur R. Jensen. Basic Books. p. 256.
- MMWR (27 May 2005). "Blood Lead Levels—United States, 1999–2002". Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. 54 (20): 513–6. PMID 15917736. Archived from the original on 5 November 2019. Retrieved 10 September 2017.
- Moore, Elsie G (May 1986). "Family socialization and the IQ test performance of traditionally and transracially adopted Black children". Developmental Psychology. 22 (3): 317–26. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.22.3.317.
- Moore, David S.; Shenk, David (2016-12-01). "The heritability fallacy". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 8 (1–2): e1400. doi:10.1002/wcs.1400. ISSN 1939-5078. PMID 27906501.
- Murray, Charles (2006). "Changes over time in the black–white difference on mental tests: Evidence from the children of the 1979 cohort of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth". Intelligence. 34 (6): 527–540. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.004.
- Murray, Charles (July–August 2007). "The magnitude and components of change in the black–white IQ difference from 1920 to 1991: A birth cohort analysis of the Woodcock–Johnson standardizations". Intelligence. 35 (4): 305–18. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2007.02.001.
- Neisser, Ulric (2004). "Serious Scientists or Disgusting Racists?". Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of Books. 49 (1): 5–7. doi:10.1037/004224.
- Neisser, Ulric; Boodoo, Gwyneth; Bouchard, Thomas J Jr; Boykin, A. Wade; Brody, Nathan; Ceci, Stephen J; Halpern, Diane F; Loehlin, John C; Perloff, Robert; Sternberg, Robert J; Urbina, Susana (1996). "Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns" (PDF). American Psychologist. 51 (2): 77–101. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2015-04-20. Retrieved 2014-08-23.
- Nevid, Jeffrey S. (2014-01-17). Essentials of Psychology: Concepts and Applications. Cengage Learning. ISBN 978-1-285-75122-1. Archived from the original on 2023-03-20. Retrieved 2018-01-30.
- Nisbett, Richard (2005). "Heredity, environment, and race differences in IQ: A commentary on Rushton and Jensen" (PDF). Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 11 (2): 302–10. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.302. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2011-06-05. Retrieved 2010-04-08.
- Nisbett, Richard (2009). Intelligence and How to Get It: Why Schools and Cultures Count. W. W. Norton & Company. ISBN 978-0-393-06505-3.
- Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (2012a). "Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments". American Psychologist. 67 (2): 130–159. doi:10.1037/a0026699. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 22233090.
- Lay summary in: "The latest on intelligence". Daniel Willingham. 2012-05-10.
- Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (2012b). "Group differences in IQ are best understood as environmental in origin" (PDF). American Psychologist. 67 (6): 503–504. doi:10.1037/a0029772. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 22963427. Archived (PDF) from the original on 23 January 2015. Retrieved 22 July 2013.
- Niu, Weihua; Brass, Jillian (2011). "Intelligence in Worldwide Perspective". In Sternberg, Robert J; Kaufmann, Scott Barry (eds.). The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-51806-2.
- Ogbu, John (1978). Minority Education and Caste: The American System in Cross-Cultural Perspective. New York: Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-125-24250-9.
- Ogbu, John U (1994). "From cultural differences to differences in cultural frames of reference". In Greenfield, PM; Cocking, RR (eds.). Cross-cultural roots of minority child development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 365–91.
- Olness, K (April 2003). "Effects on brain development leading to cognitive impairment: a worldwide epidemic". Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 24 (2): 120–30. doi:10.1097/00004703-200304000-00009. PMID 12692458. S2CID 31999992.
- Pickren, Wade E; Rutherford, Alexandra (2010). A History of Modern Psychology in Context. Wiley.
- Plotnik, Rod; Kouyoumdjian, Haig (2011). "Intelligence". Introduction to Psychology. Cengage Learning.
- Qian, M; Wang, D; Watkins, WE; Gebski, V; Yan, YQ; Li, M; Chen, ZP (2005). "The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China". Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 14 (1): 32–42. PMID 15734706.
- Reichman, Nancy E. (2005). "Low birth weight and school readiness". The Future of Children. 15 (1): 91–116. doi:10.1353/foc.2005.0008. ISSN 1054-8289. PMID 16130543. S2CID 23345980.
- Reuning, Helmut (1988). "Testing Bushmen in the Central Kalahari". In Irvine, SH; Berry, JW (eds.). Human Abilities in Cultural Context. Cambridge University Press. pp. 453–486. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511574603.018. ISBN 978-0-511-57460-3. Archived from the original on 2011-07-25. Retrieved 2010-04-26.
- Richardson, K (2004). "Book Review: IQ and the Wealth of Nations". Heredity. 92 (4): 359–60. doi:10.1038/sj.hdy.6800418.
- Rindermann, Heiner; Thompson, James (November 2013). "Ability rise in NAEP and narrowing ethnic gaps?". Intelligence. 41 (6): 821–831. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.06.016.
- Rose, Steven (2009). "Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? NO: Science and society do not benefit". Nature. 457 (7231): 786–8. Bibcode:2009Natur.457..786R. doi:10.1038/457786a. PMID 19212384. S2CID 42846614.
- Roth, PL; Bevier, CA; Bobko, P; Switzer, FS III; Tyler, P (2001). "Ethnic group differences in cognitive ability in employment and educational settings: A metaanalysis". Personnel Psychology. 54 (2): 297–330. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.372.6092. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2001.tb00094.x.
- Rouvroy, Antoinette (2008). Human genes and neoliberal governance: a Foucauldian critique. Routledge. p. 86. ISBN 978-0-415-44433-0.
- Rowe, David C (January 2005). Rodgers, Joseph (ed.). "Under the Skin: On the Impartial Treatment of Genetic and Environmental Hypothesis of Racial Differences". American Psychologist. 60 (1): 60–70. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.60. PMID 15641922.
- Rushton, J. Philippe; Jensen, Arthur R (2005). "Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability" (PDF). Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 11 (2): 246–8. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.186.102. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2015-11-03.
- Saloojee, Haroon; Pettifor, John M (December 2001). "Iron deficiency and impaired child development". BMJ. 323 (7326): 1377–8. doi:10.1136/bmj.323.7326.1377. PMC 1121846. PMID 11744547.
- Scarr, S.; Weinberg, R. A. (1990). "The nature-nurture problem revisited: The Minnesota adoption studies". Methods of family research: Biographies of research projects. Vol. 1. pp. 121–151.
- Scarr, S.; Weinberg, R. A. (1976). "IQ test performance of black children adopted by White families". American Psychologist. 31 (10): 726–739. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.31.10.726. S2CID 32410011.
- Schacter, Daniel; Gilbert, Daniel; Wegner, Daniel (2007). Psychology. New York: Worth Publishing. ISBN 978-0-7167-5215-8.
- James A. Banks (2008). "Race". In Schaefer, Richard T. (ed.). Encyclopedia of Race, Ethnicity and Society. Vol. 2. SAGE. p. 1091.
- Serpell, Robert (2000). "Intelligence and Culture". In Sternberg, Robert J. (ed.). Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge University Press. pp. 549–577. ISBN 978-0-521-59648-0.
- Sheppard, Leah D.; Vernon, Philip A. (February 2008). "Intelligence and speed of information-processing: A review of 50 years of research". Personality and Individual Differences. 44 (3): 535–551. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.015.
- Shurkin, Joel (2006). Broken Genius: The Rise and Fall of William Shockley, Creator of the Electronic Age. London: Macmillan. ISBN 978-1-4039-8815-7.
- Lay summary in: Brian Clegg. "Review - Broken Genius - Joel Shurkin". Popular Science. Archived from the original on 2006-10-06.
- Shuttleworth-Edwards, AB; Kemp, RD; Rust, AL; Muirhead, JG; Hartman, NP; Radloff, SE (October 2004). "Cross-cultural effects on IQ test performance: a review and preliminary normative indications on WAIS-III test performance". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 26 (7): 903–20. doi:10.1080/13803390490510824. PMID 15742541. S2CID 16060622.
- Smay, Diana; Armelagos, George (July 2000). "Galileo Wept: A Critical Assessment of the Use of Race in Forensic Anthropology". Transforming Anthropology. 9 (2): 19–29. doi:10.1525/tran.2000.9.2.19. S2CID 143942539.
- Snyderman, Mark; Rothman, Stanley (1987). "Survey of expert opinion on intelligence and aptitude testing". American Psychologist. 42 (2): 137–44. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.42.2.137.
- Sternberg, Robert J; Grigorenko, Elena L; Kidd, Kenneth K (2005). "Intelligence, Race, and Genetics" (PDF). American Psychologist. 60 (1): 46–59. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.174.313. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.46. PMID 15641921. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-07-20.
- Sternberg, R. J.; Grigorenko, E. L. (2007). "The Difficulty of Escaping Preconceptions in Writing an Article About the Difficulty of Escaping Preconceptions: Commentary on Hunt and Carlson (2007)". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2 (2): 221–223. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00040.x. PMID 26151963. S2CID 21096328.
- Spiro, Jonathan P. (2009). Defending the Master Race: Conservation, Eugenics, and the Legacy of Madison Grant. Univ. of Vermont Press. ISBN 978-1-58465-715-6.
- Suzuki, Lisa; Aronson, Joshua (2005). "The cultural malleability of intelligence and its impact on the racial/ethnic hierarchy". Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 11 (2): 320–327. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.1022.3693. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.320.
- Tang, Hua; Quertermous, Tom; Rodriguez, Beatriz; Kardia, Sharon L.R.; Zhu, Xiaofeng; Brown, Andrew; Pankow, James S.; Province, Michael A.; Hunt, Steven C.; Boerwinkle, Eric; Schork, Nicholas J.; Risch, Neil J. (February 2005). "Genetic structure, self-identified race/ethnicity, and confounding in case-control association studies". American Journal of Human Genetics. 76 (2): 268–75. doi:10.1086/427888. PMC 1196372. PMID 15625622.
- Templeton, A. R. (2001), "The Genetic and Evolutionary Significance of Human Races", in Fish, J. M. (ed.), Race and Intelligence Separating Science from Myth, London: Routledge, pp. 31–55, ISBN 978-0-8058-3757-5
- Thompson, Paul M.; Cannon, Tyrone D.; Narr, Katherine L.; van Erp, Theo; Poutanen, Veli-Pekka; Huttunen, Matti; Lönnqvist, Jouko; Standertskjöld-Nordenstam, Carl-Gustaf; Kaprio, Jaakko; Khaledy, Mohammad; Dail, Rajneesh; Zoumalan, Chris I.; Toga, Arthur W. (2001). "Genetic influences on brain structure" (PDF). Nature Neuroscience. 4 (12): 1253–58. doi:10.1038/nn758. PMID 11694885. S2CID 7132470. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-05-15.
- Tizard, Barbara; Cooperman, Oliver; Joseph, Anne; Tizard, Jack (June 1972). "Environmental effects on language development: A study of young children in long-stay residential nurseries". Child Development. 43 (2): 337–58. doi:10.2307/1127540. JSTOR 1127540.
- Tucker, William H (2002). The Funding of Scientific Racism: Wickliffe Draper and the Pioneer Fund. University of Illinois Press. ISBN 978-0-252-02762-8.
- Verney, Steven P.; Granholm, Eric; Marshall, Sandra P.; Malcarne, Vanessa L.; Saccuzzo, Dennis P. (26 July 2016). "Culture-Fair Cognitive Ability Assessment". Assessment. 12 (3): 303–319. doi:10.1177/1073191105276674. PMID 16123251. S2CID 31024437.
- Vincent, Ken R. (March 1991). "Black/white IQ differences: Does age make the difference?". Journal of Clinical Psychology. 47 (2): 266–270. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199103)47:2<266::aid-jclp2270470213>3.0.co;2-s. PMID 2030133.
- Weinberg, Richard A.; Scarr, Sandra; Waldman, Irwin D. (1992). "The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study: A Follow-Up of IQ Test Performance at Adolescence". Intelligence. 16 (1): 117–35. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(92)90028-P.
- Wicherts, Jelte M; Borsboom, Denny; Dolan, Conor V (2010). "Why national IQs do not support evolutionary theories of intelligence". Personality and Individual Differences. 48 (2): 91–6. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.028.
- Wicherts, Jelte M.; Borsboom, Denny; Dolan, Conor V. (2010b). "Evolution, brain size, and the national IQ of peoples around 3000 years B.C". Personality and Individual Differences. 48 (2): 104–106. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.08.020.
- Wicherts, Jelte M.; Dolan, Conor V.; Carlson, Jerry S.; van der Maas, Han L.J. (June 2010). "Raven's test performance of sub-Saharan Africans: Average performance, psychometric properties, and the Flynn Effect". Learning and Individual Differences. 20 (3): 135–151. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2009.12.001.
- Wicherts, Jelte M.; Dolan, Conor V.; van der Maas, Han L.J. (January 2010). "A systematic literature review of the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans". Intelligence. 38 (1): 1–20. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.002.
- Witelson, S. F.; Beresh, H.; Kigar, D. L. (February 2006). "Intelligence and brain size in 100 postmortem brains: sex, lateralization and age factors". Brain. 129 (2): 386–398. doi:10.1093/brain/awh696. PMID 16339797.
- Wooldridge, Adrian (1995). Measuring the Mind: Education and Psychology in England c. 1860-c. 1990. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-39515-1.
- Wroe, Andrew (2008). The Republican Party and Immigration Politics: from Proposition 187 to George W. Bush. Macmillan. p. 294. ISBN 978-0-230-60053-9.
Human intelligence topics | |
---|---|
Types | |
Abilities, traits, and constructs | |
Models and theories | |
Areas of research | |