Misplaced Pages

Race and intelligence: Difference between revisions

Article snapshot taken from Wikipedia with creative commons attribution-sharealike license. Give it a read and then ask your questions in the chat. We can research this topic together.
Browse history interactively← Previous editContent deleted Content addedVisualWikitext
Revision as of 06:44, 3 February 2020 view sourceSirfurboy (talk | contribs)Extended confirmed users21,816 edits Nominated for deletion; see Misplaced Pages:Articles for deletion/Race and intelligence (4th nomination). (TW)← Previous edit Latest revision as of 06:37, 5 January 2025 view source Citation bot (talk | contribs)Bots5,448,971 edits Added work. | Use this bot. Report bugs. | Suggested by Eastmain | #UCB_webform 307/454 
(929 intermediate revisions by more than 100 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{short description|Discussions and claims of differences in intelligence along racial lines}}
<!-- Please do not remove or change this AfD message until the issue is settled -->
{{pp-vandalism|small=yes}}
{{AfDM|page=Race and intelligence (4th nomination)|year=2020|month=February|day=3|substed=yes|origtag=afdx|help=off}}
{{pp|small=yes}}
<!-- End of AfD message, feel free to edit beyond this point -->
<noinclude>{{Race}}</noinclude>
The connection between '''race and intelligence''' has been a subject of debate in both ] and ] since the inception of ] testing in the early 20th century. There remains some debate as to whether and to what extent differences in intelligence test scores reflect environmental factors as opposed to genetic ones, as well as to the definitions of what "race" and "intelligence" are, and whether they can be objectively defined. Currently, there is no non-] that these differences in test scores have a genetic component, although some researchers believe that the existing circumstantial evidence makes it at least plausible that hard evidence for a genetic component will eventually be found.


Discussions of '''race and intelligence''' – specifically regarding claims of differences in intelligence along racial lines – have appeared in both ] and ] since the modern concept of ] was first introduced. With the inception of ] in the early 20th century, differences in average test performance between racial groups have been observed, though these differences have fluctuated and in many cases steadily decreased over time. Complicating the issue, modern science has concluded that race is a ] phenomenon rather than a biological reality, and there exist various conflicting definitions of ]. In particular, the ] as a metric for ] is disputed. Today, the scientific consensus is that ] does not explain differences in IQ test performance between groups, and that observed differences are environmental in origin.
The first test showing differences in IQ test results between different population groups in the US was the tests of United States Army recruits in World War I. In the 1920s, groups of eugenics lobbyists argued that this demonstrated that African-Americans and certain immigrant groups were of inferior intellect to Anglo-Saxon whites due to innate biological differences, using this as an argument for policies of racial segregation. Soon, other studies appeared, contesting these conclusions and arguing instead that the Army tests had not adequately controlled for environmental factors such as socio-economic and educational ].


] claims of inherent differences in intelligence between races have played a central role in the history of ]. The first tests showing differences in IQ scores between different population groups in the United States were the tests of ] recruits in ]. In the 1920s, groups of ] lobbyists argued that these results demonstrated that ] and certain immigrant groups were of inferior intellect to ] ], and that this was due to innate biological differences. In turn, they used such beliefs to justify policies of ]. However, other studies soon appeared, contesting these conclusions and arguing that the Army tests had not adequately controlled for environmental factors, such as socioeconomic and educational ].
The debate reemerged again in 1969, when ] championed the view that for genetic reasons, Africans were less intelligent than whites and that ] for African-American children was therefore doomed to be ineffective. In 1994, the book '']'' argued that social inequality in the United States could largely be explained as a result of IQ differences between races and individuals, and rekindled the public and scholarly debate with renewed force. During the debates following the book's publication, the ] and the ] (APA) published official statements regarding the issue, both highly skeptical of many of the book's claims, and the APA report called for more empirical research on the issue.


Later observations of phenomena such as the ] and disparities in access to ] highlighted ways in which environmental factors affect group IQ differences. In recent decades, as understanding of ] has advanced, claims of inherent differences in intelligence between races have been broadly rejected by scientists on both ] and ] grounds.
==History of the debate==

== History of the controversy ==
{{Main|History of the race and intelligence controversy}} {{Main|History of the race and intelligence controversy}}
{{See also|Scientific racism}} {{See also|Scientific racism}}
] and abolitionist ] (1817–1895) served as a high-profile counterexample to myths of black intellectual inferiority.]]
] (1857–1911), inventor of the first intelligence test]]
Claims of differences in intelligence between races have been used to justify ], ], ], ], and racial ]s. Claims of intellectual inferiority were used to justify British wars and colonial campaigns in Asia.<ref name="Mercer-2023">{{Cite web |last=Mercer |first=Jonathan |date=October 1, 2023 |title=Racism, Stereotypes, and War |url=https://direct.mit.edu/isec/article/48/2/7/118111/Racism-Stereotypes-and-War |access-date=2024-02-04 |website=direct.mit.edu |publisher=Journal of International Security}}</ref> Racial thinkers such as ] in France relied crucially on the assumption that black people were innately inferior to white people in developing their ideologies of ]. Even ] thinkers such as ], a slave owner, believed black people to be innately inferior to white people in physique and intellect.{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|p=23}} At the same time in the United States, prominent examples of African-American genius such the ] and abolitionist ], the pioneering sociologist ], and the poet ] stood as high-profile counterexamples to widespread stereotypes of black intellectual inferiority.<ref name="LawsonKirkland1999">Stewart, Roderick M. 1999. "The Claims of Frederick Douglass Philosophically Considered." Pp. 155–56 in ''Frederick Douglass: A Critical Reader'', edited by B. E. Lawson and F. M. Kirkland. Wiley-Blackwell. {{ISBN|978-0-631-20578-4}}. "Moreover, though he does not make the point explicitly, again the very fact that Douglass is ably disputing this argument on this occasion celebrating a select few's intellect and will (or moral character)—this fact constitutes a living counterexample to the narrowness of the pro-slavery definition of humans."</ref><ref>Marable, Manning (2011), ''Living Black History: How Reimagining the African-American Past Can Remake America's Racial Future'', p. 96. {{ISBN|978-0-465-04395-8}}.</ref> In Britain, Japan's military victory over Russia in the ]<ref name="Mercer-2023" /> began to reverse negative stereotypes of "oriental" inferiority.<ref name="Tonooka-2017">{{Cite journal |last=Tonooka |first=Chika |date=2017 |title=Reverse Emulation and the Cult of Japanese Efficiency in Edwardian Britain |url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/26343378 |journal=The Historical Journal |volume=60 |issue=1 |pages=95–119 |doi=10.1017/S0018246X15000539 |jstor=26343378 |s2cid=162698331 |issn=0018-246X}}</ref> ] (1857–1911), inventor of the first intelligence test|alt=|left]]


=== Early IQ testing ===
Claims of races having different intelligence were used to justify ], ], ], ], and racial ]s. Racial thinkers such as ] relied crucially on the assumption that black people were innately inferior to whites in developing their ideologies of ]. Even enlightenment thinkers such as ], a slave owner, believed blacks to be innately inferior to whites in physique and intellect.{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|p=23}}
The first practical intelligence test, the ], was developed between 1905 and 1908 by ] and ] in France for school placement of children. Binet warned that results from his test should not be assumed to measure innate intelligence or used to label individuals permanently.{{sfn|Plotnik|Kouyoumdjian|2011}} Binet's test was translated into English and revised in 1916 by ] (who introduced IQ scoring for the test results) and published under the name ]. In 1916 Terman wrote that Mexican-Americans, African-Americans, and Native Americans have a mental "dullness seems to be racial, or at least inherent in the family stocks from which they come."<ref>{{cite book |last=Terman |first=Lewis |title=The Measurement Of Intelligence |publisher=Houghton, Mifflin and Company |year=1916 |page=91 |oclc=557712625}}</ref>


The US Army used a different set of tests developed by ] to evaluate draftees for World War I. Based on the Army's data, prominent psychologists and eugenicists such as ], ], and Princeton professor ] wrote that people from southern and eastern Europe were less intelligent than native-born Americans or immigrants from the Nordic countries, and that black Americans were less intelligent than white Americans.{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|page=116}} The results were widely publicized by a lobby of anti-immigration activists, including the conservationist and theorist of ] ], who considered the so-called ] to be superior, but under threat because of immigration by "inferior breeds." In his influential work, ''A Study of American Intelligence,'' psychologist ] used the results of the Army tests to argue for a stricter immigration policy, limiting immigration to countries considered to belong to the "Nordic race".{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|pages=116, 309}}
===Early IQ testing===
The first practical intelligence test was developed between 1905 and 1908 by ] in France for school placement of children. Binet warned that results from his test should not be assumed to measure innate intelligence or used to label individuals permanently.{{sfn|Plotnik|Kouyoumdjian|2011}} Binet's test was translated into English and revised in 1916 by ] (who introduced IQ scoring for the test results) and published under the name the ]. As Terman's test was published, there was great concern in the United States about the abilities and skills of recent immigrants. Different immigrant nationalities were sometimes thought to belong to different races, such as ]. A different set of tests developed by ] were used to evaluate draftees for World War I, and researchers found that people from southern and eastern Europe scored lower than native-born Americans, that Americans from northern states had higher scores than Americans from southern states, and that black Americans scored lower than white Americans.{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|page=116}} The results were widely publicized by a lobby of anti-immigration activists, including the New York patrician and conservationist ], who considered the ] to be superior, but under threat of immigration by inferior breeds. In his influential work, ''A Study of American Intelligence,'' psychologist ] used the results of the Army tests to argue for a stricter immigration policy, limiting immigration to countries considered to belong to the "Nordic race".{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|page=116, 309}}


In the 1920s, states like Virginia enacted ] laws, such as its ], which established the ] as law. Many scientists reacted negatively to eugenicist claims linking abilities and moral character to racial or genetic ancestry. They pointed to the contribution of environment to test results (such as speaking English as a second language).{{sfn|Pickren|Rutherford|2010|p=163}} By the mid-1930s, many United States psychologists adopted the view that environmental and cultural factors played a dominant role in IQ test results, among them Carl Brigham, who repudiated his own previous arguments on the grounds that he realized that the tests were not a measure of innate intelligence. Discussion of the issue in the United States also influenced ] ] claims of the "Nordics" being a "]", influenced by Grant's writings.{{sfn|Spiro|2009}} As the American public sentiment shifted against the Germans, claims of racial differences in intelligence increasingly came to be regarded as problematic.<ref name="Ludy 2006">{{harvnb|Ludy|2006}}</ref> Anthropologists such as ], and ] and ], did much to demonstrate the unscientific status of many of the claims about racial hierarchies of intelligence.{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|pages=130–32}} Nonetheless, a powerful eugenics and segregation lobby funded largely by textile-magnate ] continued to publicize studies using intelligence studies as an argument for eugenics, segregation, and anti-immigration legislation.{{sfn|Tucker|2002}} In the 1920s, some US states enacted ] laws, such as Virginia's ], which established the ] (of ']') as law. Many scientists reacted negatively to eugenicist claims linking abilities and moral character to racial or genetic ancestry. They pointed to the contribution of environment (such as speaking English as a second language) to test results.{{sfn|Pickren|Rutherford|2010|p=163}} By the mid-1930s, many psychologists in the US had adopted the view that environmental and cultural factors played a dominant role in IQ test results. The psychologist Carl Brigham repudiated his own earlier arguments, explaining that he had come to realize that the tests were not a measure of innate intelligence.{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|page=145}}


Discussions of the issue in the United States, especially in the writings of Madison Grant, influenced ] ] claims that the "Nordics" were a "]."{{sfn|Spiro|2009}} As American public sentiment shifted against the Germans, claims of racial differences in intelligence increasingly came to be regarded as problematic.<ref name="Ludy 2006">{{harvnb|Ludy|2006}}</ref> Anthropologists such as ], ], and ] did much to demonstrate that claims about racial hierarchies of intelligence were unscientific.{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|pages=130–32}} Nonetheless, a powerful eugenics and segregation lobby funded largely by textile-magnate ] continued to use intelligence studies as an argument for eugenics, segregation, and anti-immigration legislation.{{sfn|Tucker|2002}}
===The Jensenism debates===
{{See also|Jensenism}}
As the de-segregation of the American South gained traction in the 1950s, the debate about black intelligence resurfaced. ], funded by Draper's ], published a new analysis of Yerkes' tests, concluding that blacks really were of inferior intellect to whites. This study was used by segregationists as an argument that it was to the advantage of black children to be educated separately from the superior white children.{{sfn|Jackson|2005}} In the 1960s, the debate was further revived when ] publicly defended the argument that black children were innately unable to learn as well as white children.{{sfn|Shurkin|2006}} ] stimulated scholarly discussion of the issue with his '']'' article, "]"{{sfn|Jensen|1969|p=82}}{{sfn|Tucker|2002}}{{sfn|Wooldridge|1995}}<ref>{{cite book |last1=Panofsky |first1=Aaron |title=Misbehaving Science. Controversy and the Development of Behavior Genetics |publisher=] |location=Chicago |isbn=978-0-226-05831-3 |date=2014}}</ref> Jensen's article questioned remedial education for African-American children; he suggested their poor educational performance reflected an underlying genetic cause rather than lack of stimulation at home.{{sfn|Alland|2002|pages=79–80}} Jensen continued to publish on the issue until his death in 2012.


===The ''Bell Curve'' debate=== === The Pioneer Fund and ''The Bell Curve'' ===
As the desegregation of the American South gained traction in the 1950s, debate about black intelligence resurfaced. ], funded by Draper's ], published a new analysis of Yerkes' tests, concluding that black people really were of inferior intellect to white people. This study was used by segregationists to argue that it was to the advantage of black children to be educated separately from the superior white children.{{sfn|Jackson|2005}} In the 1960s, the debate was revived when ] publicly defended the view that black children were innately unable to learn as well as white children.{{sfn|Shurkin|2006}} ] expressed similar opinions in his '']'' article, "]," which questioned the value of ] for African-American children.{{sfn|Jensen|1969|pages=1–123}} He suggested that poor educational performance in such cases reflected an underlying genetic cause rather than lack of stimulation at home or other environmental factors.<ref>{{cite book |last1=Panofsky |first1=Aaron |title=Misbehaving Science. Controversy and the Development of Behavior Genetics |publisher=] |location=Chicago |isbn=978-0-226-05831-3 |date=2014}}</ref>{{sfn|Alland|2002|pages=79–80}}
Another revival of public debate followed the appearance of '']'' (1994), a book by ] and ], who strongly emphasized the societal effects of low IQ (focusing in most chapters strictly on the non-Hispanic white population of the United States).{{sfn|Herrnstein|Murray|1994}} In 1994, a group of 52 researchers (mostly psychologists) signed an editorial statement "]" in response to the book. ''The Bell Curve'' also led to a 1995 report from the ], "]", acknowledging a difference between mean IQ scores of whites and blacks as well as the absence of any adequate explanation of it, either environmental or genetic. ''The Bell Curve'' prompted the publication of several multiple-author books responding from a variety of points of view.<ref name="Mackintosh 1998">{{harvnb|Mackintosh|1998}}</ref><ref name="Maltby, Day & Macaskill 2007"/> They include '']'' (1995), '']'' (1996) and a second edition of '']'' (1996) by ].<ref name="Maltby, Day & Macaskill 2007"/> Jensen's last book-length publication, '']'', was published a few years later in 1998.


Another revival of public debate followed the appearance of '']'' (1994), a book by ] and ] that supported the general viewpoint of Jensen.{{sfn|Herrnstein|Murray|1994}} A statement in support of Herrnstein and Murray titled "]," was published in '']'' with 52 signatures. ''The Bell Curve'' also led to critical responses in a statement titled "]" of the American Psychological Association and in several books, including '']'' (1995), '']'' (1996) and a second edition of '']'' (1996) by ].<ref name="Maltby, Day & Macaskill 2007"/><ref name="Mackintosh 1998">{{harvnb|Mackintosh|1998}}</ref>
The review article "Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability" by ] and Jensen was published in 2005.{{sfn|Rushton|Jensen|2005}} The article was followed by a series of responses, some in support, some critical.<ref name="Ludy 2006"/><ref>{{harvnb|Rouvroy|2008|p=86}}</ref> ], another psychologist who had also commented at the time, later included an amplified version of his critique as part of the book ''Intelligence and How to Get It: Why Schools and Cultures Count'' (2009).<ref>{{harvnb|Nisbett|2009|pp=209–36}}</ref> Rushton and Jensen in 2010 made a point-by-point reply to this thereafter.<ref name="Rushton & Jensen 2010">{{harvnb|Rushton|Jensen|2010}}</ref> A comprehensive review article on the issue was published in the journal ''American Psychologist'' in 2012.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012}}


Some of the authors proposing genetic explanations for group differences have received funding from the ], which was headed by Rushton until his death in 2012.{{sfn|Tucker|2002}}<ref name="Maltby, Day & Macaskill 2007">{{harvnb|Maltby|Day|Macaskill|2007}}</ref>{{sfn|Graves|2002a}}{{sfn|Graves|2002b}}<ref>{{harvnb|Grossman|Kaufman|2001}}</ref> The ] lists the Pioneer Fund as a ], citing the fund's history, its funding of race and intelligence research, and its connections with ] individuals.{{sfn|Berlet|2003}} Other researchers have criticized the Pioneer Fund for promoting ], ] and ].{{sfn|Tucker|2002}}<ref> {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110525150639/http://www.pioneerfund.org/Board.html |date=2011-05-25 }}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Falk|2008|p=18}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Wroe|2008|p=81}}</ref> Some of the authors proposing genetic explanations for group differences have received funding from the ], which was headed by ] until his death in 2012.{{sfn|Tucker|2002}}<ref name="Maltby, Day & Macaskill 2007">{{harvnb|Maltby|Day|Macaskill|2007}}</ref>{{sfn|Graves|2002a}}{{sfn|Graves|2002b}}<ref>{{harvnb|Grossman|Kaufman|2001}}</ref> Arthur Jensen, who jointly with Rushton published a 2005 review article arguing that the difference in average IQs between blacks and whites is partly due to genetics, received $1.1 million in grants from the Pioneer Fund.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Adam |first1=Miller |year=1994 |title=The Pioneer Fund: Bankrolling the Professors of Hate |url=https://www.jstor.org/stable/2962466 |journal=The Journal of Blacks in Higher Education |issue=6 |pages=58–61 |doi=10.2307/2962466|jstor=2962466 }}</ref><ref>{{cite web |last1=Blakemore |first1=Bill |last2=Jennings |first2=Peter |last3=Nissen |first3=Beth |date=November 22, 1994 |title=The Bell Curve and the Pioneer Fund |url=http://www.ferris.edu/isar/tanton/abcnews.htm |work=ABC World News Tonight |publisher=ABC News |access-date=May 1, 2020 |archive-date=March 3, 2016 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160303213542/http://www.ferris.edu/isar/tanton/abcnews.htm |url-status=live }} Vanderbilt Television News Archive : {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160103223437/http://tvnews.vanderbilt.edu/program.pl?ID=151406 |date=January 3, 2016 }}</ref> According to ], "The University of California's Arthur Jensen, cited twenty-three times in ''The Bell Curve''{{'}}s bibliography, is the book's principal authority on the intellectual inferiority of blacks."<ref>{{cite book |last=Montagu |first=Ashley |title=Race and IQ |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=New York |year=2002 |edition=2 |isbn=978-0-19-510221-5}}</ref>


The ] lists the Pioneer Fund as a ], citing the fund's history, its funding of race and intelligence research, and its connections with ] individuals.{{sfn|Berlet|2003}} Other researchers have criticized the Pioneer Fund for promoting ], ] and ].{{sfn|Tucker|2002}}<ref> {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110525150639/http://www.pioneerfund.org/Board.html |date=2011-05-25 }}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Falk|2008|p=18}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Wroe|2008|p=81}}</ref>
==Validity of race and IQ==


== Conceptual issues ==
===Intelligence, IQ, ''g'' and IQ tests===
{{Main|Intelligence quotient|Intelligence}}
The concept of intelligence and the degree to which intelligence is measurable is a matter of debate. While there is some consensus about how to define intelligence, it is not universally accepted that it is something that can be unequivocally measured by a single figure.<ref name="Schacter, Gilbert & Wegner 2007, pp 350-1">{{harvnb|Schacter|Gilbert|Wegner|2007|pp=350–1}}</ref> A recurring criticism is that different societies value and promote different kinds of skills and that the concept of intelligence is therefore culturally variable and cannot be measured by the same criteria in different societies.<ref name="Schacter, Gilbert & Wegner 2007, pp 350-1"/> Consequently, some critics argue that proposed relationships to other variables are necessarily tentative.<ref name="Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd 2005">{{harvnb|Sternberg|Grigorenko|Kidd|2005}}</ref>


=== Intelligence and IQ ===
In relation to the study of racial differences in IQ test scores, it becomes a crucial question of what exactly it is that IQ tests measure. Arthur Jensen was a proponent of the view that there is a correlation between scores on all the known types of IQ tests, and that this correlation points to an underlying factor of ], or ''g''. In most conceptions of ''g,'' it is considered to be fairly fixed in a given individual and unresponsive to training or other environmental influences. In this view, test score differences, especially in those tasks considered to be particularly "g-loaded", reflect the test taker's innate capability.
{{Main|Human intelligence|Intelligence quotient|G factor (psychometrics)}}
The concept of intelligence and the degree to which intelligence is measurable are matters of debate. There is no consensus about how to define intelligence; nor is it universally accepted that it is something that can be meaningfully measured by a single figure.<ref name="Schacter, Gilbert & Wegner 2007, pp 350-1">{{harvnb|Schacter|Gilbert|Wegner|2007|pp=350–1}}</ref> A recurring criticism is that different societies value and promote different kinds of skills and that the concept of intelligence is therefore culturally variable and cannot be measured by the same criteria in different societies.<ref name="Schacter, Gilbert & Wegner 2007, pp 350-1"/> Consequently, some critics argue that it makes no sense to propose relationships between intelligence and other variables.<ref name="Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd 2005">{{harvnb|Sternberg|Grigorenko|Kidd|2005}}</ref>


Correlations between scores on various types of IQ tests led English psychologist ] to propose in 1904 the existence of an underlying factor, which he referred to as "''g''" or "]", a trait which is supposed to be innate.<ref name="deary2008">{{Cite journal |last1=Deary |first1=I. J. |last2=Lawn |first2=M. |last3=Bartholomew |first3=D. J. |year=2008 |title="A conversation between Charles Spearman, Godfrey Thomson, and Edward L. Thorndike: The International Examinations Inquiry Meetings 1931-1938": Correction to Deary, Lawn, and Bartholomew (2008) |url=https://www.research.ed.ac.uk/portal/files/8897614/a_conversation_between_charles_spareman.pdf |journal=History of Psychology |volume=11 |issue=3 |pages=156–157 |doi=10.1037/1093-4510.11.3.163 |hdl=20.500.11820/5417f3c7-e873-40b9-ad73-19c6acc9e35b |access-date=2020-06-25 |archive-date=2020-08-06 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200806163233/https://www.research.ed.ac.uk/portal/files/8897614/a_conversation_between_charles_spareman.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref> Another proponent of this view is ].{{sfn|Jensen|1998||page=}} This view, however, has been contradicted by a number of studies showing that education and changes in environment can significantly improve IQ test results.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Ceci |first=Stephen J. |date=1991 |title=How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence |journal=Developmental Psychology |volume=27 |issue=5 |pages=703–722 |doi=10.1037/0012-1649.27.5.703}}</ref>{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}}<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Richie |first1=Stuart J. |last2=Tucker-Drob |first2=Elliot |date=June 2018 |title=How Much Does Education Improve Intelligence? A Meta-Analysis |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/325832102 |journal=Psychological Science |volume=29 |issue=8}}</ref>
Other psychometricians argue that, while there may or may not be a general intelligence factor, performance on tests relies crucially on knowledge acquired through prior exposure to the types of tasks that such tests contain. This view would mean that tests cannot be expected to reflect only the innate abilities of a given individual, because the expression of potential will always be mediated by experience and cognitive habits. It also means that comparison of test scores from persons with widely different life experiences and cognitive habits is not an expression of their relative innate potentials.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|p=359}}


Other psychometricians have argued that, whether or not there is such a thing as a general intelligence factor, performance on tests relies crucially on knowledge acquired through prior exposure to the types of tasks that such tests contain. This means that comparisons of test scores between persons with widely different life experiences and cognitive habits do not reveal their relative innate potentials.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|p=359}}
===Race===
{{Main|Race (classification of humans)|Race and genetics}}
The majority of anthropologists today consider race to be a sociopolitical phenomenon rather than a biological one,{{sfn|Daley|Onwuegbuzie|2011|p=294}} a view supported by considerable genetics research.{{sfn|Smay|Armelagos|2000}}<ref>{{Cite journal| journal = Nature Genetics |date=2004 | volume = 36 |issue=11 Suppl | pages = 43–47 |author1=Rotimi, Charles N. | title = Are medical and nonmedical uses of large-scale genomic markers conflating genetics and 'race'? | doi=10.1038/ng1439 |quote="Two facts are relevant: (i) as a result of different evolutionary forces, including natural selection, there are geographical patterns of genetic variations that correspond, for the most part, to continental origin; and (ii) observed patterns of geographical differences in genetic information do not correspond to our notion of social identities, including 'race' and 'ethnicity" | pmid=15508002}}</ref> The current mainstream view in the social sciences and biology is that race is a social construction based on folk ideologies that construct groups based on social disparities and superficial physical characteristics.<ref>{{harvnb|Schaefer|2008}}</ref> {{harvp|Sternberg|Grigorenko|Kidd|2005}} state, "Race is a socially constructed concept, not a biological one. It derives from people's desire to classify."<ref name="Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd 2005"/> The concept of human "races" as natural and separate divisions within the human species has also been rejected by the ]. The official position of the AAA, adopted in 1998, is that advances in scientific knowledge have made it "clear that human populations are not unambiguous, clearly demarcated, biologically distinct groups" and that "any attempt to establish lines of division among biological populations both arbitrary and subjective."<ref name="AAA">{{harvnb|AAA|1998}}</ref>


=== Race ===
Race in studies of human intelligence is almost always determined using self-reports, rather than based on analyses of the genetic characteristics of the tested individuals. According to psychologist David Rowe, self-report is the preferred method for racial classification in studies of racial differences because classification based on genetic markers alone ignore the "cultural, behavioral, sociological, psychological, and epidemiological variables" that distinguish racial groups.<ref name="Rowe 2005">{{harvnb|Rowe|2005}}</ref> Hunt and Carlson write that "Nevertheless, self-identification is a surprisingly reliable guide to genetic composition. {{harvp|Tang et al.|2005}} applied mathematical clustering techniques to sort genomic markers for over 3,600 people in the United States and Taiwan into four groups. There was almost perfect agreement between cluster assignment and individuals' self-reports of racial/ethnic identification as white, black, East Asian, or Latino."<ref name="Hunt & Carlson 2007"/> Sternberg and Grigorenko disagree with Hunt and Carlson's interpretation of Tang, "Tang et al.'s point was that ancient geographic ancestry rather than current residence is associated with self-identification and not that such self-identification provides evidence for the existence of biological race."<ref>{{harvnb|Sternberg|Grigorenko|2007}}</ref>
{{Main|Race (human categorization)|Race and genetics}}
The consensus view among geneticists, biologists and anthropologists is that race is a sociopolitical phenomenon rather than a biological one,<ref name="NASEM-2023">{{Cite book |url=https://nap.nationalacademies.org/read/26902/chapter/1 |title=Using Population Descriptors in Genetics and Genomics Research: A New Framework for an Evolving Field (Consensus Study Report) |date=2023 |publisher=] |doi=10.17226/26902 |pmid=36989389 |isbn=978-0-309-70065-8 |quote=In humans, race is a socially constructed designation, a misleading and harmful surrogate for population genetic differences, and has a long history of being incorrectly identified as the major genetic reason for phenotypic differences between groups.}}</ref>{{sfn|Daley|Onwuegbuzie|2011|p=294}}<ref name="Templeton2016">Templeton, A. (2016). EVOLUTION AND NOTIONS OF HUMAN RACE. In Losos J. & Lenski R. (Eds.), ''How Evolution Shapes Our Lives: Essays on Biology and Society'' (pp. 346–361). Princeton; Oxford: Princeton University Press. {{doi|10.2307/j.ctv7h0s6j.26}}. That this view reflects the consensus among American anthropologists is stated in: {{cite journal |last1=Wagner |first1=Jennifer K. |last2=Yu |first2=Joon-Ho |last3=Ifekwunigwe |first3=Jayne O. |last4=Harrell |first4=Tanya M. |last5=Bamshad |first5=Michael J. |last6=Royal |first6=Charmaine D. |date=February 2017 |title=Anthropologists' views on race, ancestry, and genetics |journal=American Journal of Physical Anthropology |volume=162 |issue=2 |pages=318–327 |doi=10.1002/ajpa.23120 |pmc=5299519 |pmid=27874171}} See also: {{cite web |author=] |date=27 March 2019 |title=AAPA Statement on Race and Racism |url=https://physanth.org/about/position-statements/aapa-statement-race-and-racism-2019/ |access-date=19 June 2020 |website=American Association of Physical Anthropologists |archive-date=25 January 2022 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20220125163036/https://physanth.org/about/position-statements/aapa-statement-race-and-racism-2019/ |url-status=live }}</ref> a view supported by considerable genetics research.{{sfn|Smay|Armelagos|2000}}<ref>{{Cite journal |journal=Nature Genetics |date=2004 |volume=36 |issue=11 Suppl |pages=43–47 |author1=Rotimi, Charles N. |title=Are medical and nonmedical uses of large-scale genomic markers conflating genetics and 'race'? |doi=10.1038/ng1439 |quote="Two facts are relevant: (i) as a result of different evolutionary forces, including natural selection, there are geographical patterns of genetic variations that correspond, for the most part, to continental origin; and (ii) observed patterns of geographical differences in genetic information do not correspond to our notion of social identities, including 'race' and 'ethnicity" |pmid=15508002 |doi-access=free}}</ref> The current mainstream view is that race is a social construction based on folk ideologies that construct groups based on social disparities and superficial physical characteristics.<ref>{{harvnb|Schaefer|2008}}</ref> A 2023 consensus report from the ] stated: "In humans, race is a socially constructed designation, a misleading and harmful surrogate for population genetic differences, and has a long history of being incorrectly identified as the major genetic reason for phenotypic differences between groups."<ref name="NASEM-2023" />


The concept of human "races" as natural and separate divisions within the human species has also been rejected by the ]. The official position of the AAA, adopted in 1998, is that advances in scientific knowledge have made it "clear that human populations are not unambiguous, clearly demarcated, biologically distinct groups" and that "any attempt to establish lines of division among biological populations both arbitrary and subjective."<ref name="AAA">{{harvnb|AAA|1998}}</ref> A more recent statement from the ] (2019) declares that "Race does not provide an accurate representation of human biological variation. It was never accurate in the past, and it remains inaccurate when referencing contemporary human populations. Humans are not divided biologically into distinct continental types or racial genetic clusters."<ref>{{Cite web |title=AAPA Statement on Race & Racism |url=https://physanth.org/about/position-statements/aapa-statement-race-and-racism-2019/ |access-date=2020-06-28 |archive-date=2022-01-25 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20220125163036/https://physanth.org/about/position-statements/aapa-statement-race-and-racism-2019/ |url-status=live }}</ref>
Anthropologist ]<ref name="Brace 2005">{{harvnb|Brace|2005}}</ref> and geneticist ] disagree with the idea that cluster analysis and the correlation between self-reported race and genetic ancestry support biological race.<ref>{{harvnb|Graves|2001}}</ref> They argue that while it is possible to find biological and genetic variation corresponding roughly to the groupings normally defined as races, this is true for almost all geographically distinct populations. The cluster structure of the genetic data is dependent on the initial hypotheses of the researcher and the populations sampled. When one samples continental groups, the clusters become continental; if one had chosen other sampling patterns, the clusters would be different. {{harvnb|Kaplan|2011}} therefore concludes that, while differences in particular allele frequencies can be used to identify populations that loosely correspond to the racial categories common in Western social discourse, the differences are of no more biological significance than the differences found between any human populations (e.g., the Spanish and Portuguese).


Anthropologists such as ],<ref name="Brace 2005">{{harvnb|Brace|2005}}</ref> the philosophers Jonathan Kaplan and Rasmus Winther,<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Kaplan |first1=Jonathan Michael |last2=Winther |first2=Rasmus Grønfeldt |date=2014 |title=Realism, Antirealism, and Conventionalism About Race |url=https://philpapers.org/rec/KAPRAA |journal=] |volume=81 |issue=5 |pages=1039–1052 |doi=10.1086/678314 |s2cid=55148854}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last=Winther |first=Rasmus Grønfeldt |date=2015 |title=The Genetic Reification of 'Race'?: A Story of Two Mathematical Methods |url=http://philpapers.org/archive/WINTGR.pdf |journal=] |volume=2 |issue=2 |pages=204–223}}</ref>{{sfnp|Kaplan|Winther|2013}} and the geneticist ],<ref>{{Cite web |last=Graves |first=Joseph |date=7 June 2006 |title=What We Know and What We Don't Know: Human Genetic Variation and the Social Construction of Race |url=http://raceandgenomics.ssrc.org/Graves/ |website=Race and Genomics |access-date=3 December 2023 |archive-date=3 June 2019 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190603030227/http://raceandgenomics.ssrc.org/Graves/ |url-status=dead }}</ref> have argued that the cluster structure of genetic data is dependent on the initial hypotheses of the researcher and the influence of these hypotheses on the choice of populations to sample. When one samples continental groups, the clusters become continental, but if one had chosen other sampling patterns, the clustering would be different. Weiss and Fullerton have noted that if one sampled only Icelanders, Mayans and Maoris, three distinct clusters would form and all other populations could be described as being clinally composed of admixtures of Maori, Icelandic and Mayan genetic materials.<ref name="evolutionary">{{cite journal |last1=Weiss |first1=K. M. |last2=Fullerton |first2=S. M. |date=2005 |title=Racing around, getting nowhere |journal=Evolutionary Anthropology |volume=14 |issue=5 |pages=165–169 |doi=10.1002/evan.20079 |s2cid=84927946}}</ref> Kaplan and Winther conclude that while racial groups are characterized by different allele frequencies, this does not mean that racial classification is a natural taxonomy of the human species, because multiple other genetic patterns can be found in human populations that crosscut racial distinctions. Moreover, the genomic data underdetermines whether one ]. Under Kaplan and Winther's view, racial groupings are objective social constructions (see Mills 1998<ref>{{cite book |last=Mills |first=C. W. |title=Blackness visible: essays on philosophy and race |date=1988 |publisher=] |location=Ithaca, New York |pages=41–66 |chapter=But What Are You Really? The Metaphysics of Race |author-link=C. Wright Mills}}</ref>) that have conventional biological reality only insofar as the categories are chosen and constructed for pragmatic scientific reasons. {{harvp|Sternberg|Grigorenko|Kidd|2005}} argue that the social construction of race derives not from any valid scientific basis but rather "from people's desire to classify."<ref name="Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd 2005" />
] agrees that racial categories are defined by social conventions, though he points out that they also correlate with clusters of both genetic traits and cultural traits. Hunt explains that, due to this, racial IQ differences are caused by these variables that correlate with race, and race itself is rarely a causal variable. Researchers who study racial disparities in test scores are studying the relationship between the scores and the many race-related factors which could potentially affect performance. These factors include health, wealth, biological differences, and education.<ref name="Hunt 2010, pp 408–10">{{harvnb|Hunt|2010|pp=408–10}}</ref>


In studies of human intelligence, race is almost always determined using self-reports rather than analyses of genetic characteristics. According to psychologist David Rowe, self-report is the preferred method for racial classification in studies of racial differences because classification based on genetic markers alone ignore the "cultural, behavioral, sociological, psychological, and epidemiological variables" that distinguish racial groups.<ref name="Rowe 2005">{{harvnb|Rowe|2005}}</ref> Hunt and Carlson disagreed, writing that "Nevertheless, self-identification is a surprisingly reliable guide to genetic composition," citing a study by {{harvp|Tang et al.|2005}}.<ref name="Hunt & Carlson 2007">{{harvnb|Hunt|Carlson|2007}}</ref> Sternberg and Grigorenko disputed Hunt and Carlson's interpretation of Tang's results as supporting the view that racial divisions are biological; rather, "Tang et al.'s point was that ancient geographic ancestry rather than current residence is associated with self-identification and not that such self-identification provides evidence for the existence of biological race."<ref>{{harvnb|Sternberg|Grigorenko|2007}}</ref>
==Group differences==
The study of human intelligence is one of the most controversial topics in psychology. It remains unclear whether group differences in intelligence test scores are caused by heritable factors or by "other correlated demographic variables such as ] status, ], and motivation."<ref>{{Harvnb|Hampshire|Highfield|Parkin|Owen|2012}}.</ref>
Hunt and Carlson outlined four contemporary positions on differences in IQ based on race or ethnicity. The first is that these reflect real differences in average group intelligence, which is caused by a combination of environmental factors and heritable differences in brain function. A second position is that differences in average cognitive ability between races are caused entirely by social and/or environmental factors. A third position holds that differences in average cognitive ability between races do not exist, and that the differences in average test scores are the result of inappropriate use of the tests themselves. Finally, a fourth position is that either or both of the concepts of ] and ] are poorly constructed and therefore any comparisons between races are meaningless.<ref name="Hunt & Carlson 2007"/>


== Group differences ==
===United States test scores===
The study of human intelligence is one of the most controversial topics in psychology, in part because of difficulty reaching agreement about the meaning of ''intelligence'' and objections to the assumption that intelligence can be meaningfully measured by IQ tests. Claims that there are innate differences in intelligence between racial and ethnic groups—which go back at least to the 19th century—have been criticized for relying on specious assumptions and research methods and for serving as an ideological framework for discrimination and racism.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}}{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|p=222}}
{{main|Achievement gap in the United States}}
In the United States, individuals identifying themselves as East Asian tend to have higher average IQ scores than do Caucasians, who, in turn, have higher average IQs than African Americans. Nevertheless, greater variation in IQ scores exists within each ethnic group than between them.<ref>{{Cite book |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=yDDqLBBk7BcC |title=Encyclopedia of Diversity in Education |date=2012 |publisher=SAGE |isbn=9781412981521 |pages=1209 |language=en}}</ref>


In a 2012 study of tests of different components of intelligence, Hampshire et al. expressed disagreement with the view of Jensen and Rushton that genetic factors must play a role in IQ differences between races, stating that "it remains unclear ... whether population differences in intelligence test scores are driven by heritable factors or by other correlated demographic variables such as socioeconomic status, education level, and motivation. More relevantly, it is questionable whether relate to a unitary intelligence factor, as opposed to a bias in testing paradigms toward particular components of a more complex intelligence construct."<ref name=":1">{{Harvnb|Hampshire|Highfield|Parkin|Owen|2012}}.</ref> According to Jackson and Weidman,
In response to the controversial 1994 book '']'', the ] (APA) formed a task-force of eleven experts, which issued a report, "]" in 1995. Regarding group differences, the report reaffirmed the consensus that differences within groups are much wider than difference between groups, and that that claims of ethnic difference in intelligence should be scrutinized carefully, as this had been used to justify racial discrimination. It also acknowledged limitations in the racial categories used, as these categories are neither consistently applied, nor homogeneous (see also ]).<ref name="Neisser et al. 1996"/>
{{blockquote|There are a number of reasons why the genetic argument for race differences in intelligence has not won many adherents in the scientific community. First, even taken on its own terms, the case made by Jensen and his followers did not hold up to scrutiny. Second, the rise of population genetics undercut the claims for a genetic cause of intelligence. Third, the new understanding of ] offered a better explanation for the existence of differences in IQ scores between the races.{{sfn|Jackson|Weidman|2004|p=222}}}}


=== Test scores ===
{{harvp|Roth et al.|2001}}, in a review of the results of a total of 6,246,729 participants on other tests of cognitive ability or aptitude, found a difference in mean IQ scores between blacks and whites of 1.1 SD. Consistent results were found for college and university application tests such as the ] (N = 2.4 million) and ] (N = 2.3 million), as well as for tests of job applicants in corporate sections (N = 0.5 million) and in the military (N = 0.4 million).<ref name="Roth et al. 2001">{{harvnb|Roth et al.|2001}}</ref> According to the same study, ]ns have tended to score relatively higher on visuospatial subtests with lower scores in verbal subtests while ] with lower scores in visuospatial subtests. The few ] populations who have been systematically tested, including Arctic Natives, tend to score worse on average than white populations but better on average than black populations.<ref name="Roth et al. 2001"/>{{dubious|Neutral summary of older sources|date=July 2019}}
{{main|Achievement gap in the United States}}

In the United States, Asians on average score higher than White people, who tend to score higher than Hispanics, who tend to score higher than African Americans.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} Much greater variation in IQ scores exists within each ethnic group than between them.{{Clarify|reason=The prose here should be clarified. Is it saying that the recorded IQ range within each race is greater than any differences of averages between races?|date=December 2023}}<ref name="Reynolds-2021">{{Cite book |last1=Reynolds |first1=Cecil R. |chapter-url=https://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-3-030-59455-8_15 |title=Mastering Modern Psychological Testing |last2=Altmann |first2=Robert A. |last3=Allen |first3=Daniel N. |publisher=Springer |year=2021 |pages=573–613, 582 |chapter=The Problem of Bias in Psychological Assessment|doi=10.1007/978-3-030-59455-8_15 |isbn=978-3-030-59454-1 |s2cid=236660997 }}</ref><ref name=SAGE>{{Cite book |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=yDDqLBBk7BcC |title=Encyclopedia of Diversity in Education |date=2012 |publisher=SAGE |isbn=978-1-4129-8152-1 |page=1209 |language=en |access-date=2018-01-20 |archive-date=2023-03-20 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230320043631/https://books.google.com/books?id=yDDqLBBk7BcC |url-status=live }}</ref> A 2001 ] of the results of 6,246,729 participants tested for cognitive ability or aptitude found a difference in average scores between black people and white people of 1.1 ]. Consistent results were found for college and university application tests such as the ] (N = 2.4 million) and ] (N = 2.3 million), as well as for tests of job applicants in corporate settings (N = 0.5 million) and in the military (N = 0.4 million).<ref name="Roth et al. 2001">{{harvnb|Roth et al.|2001}}</ref>
The racial groups studied in the United States and Europe are not necessarily ] for populations in other parts of the world. Cultural differences may also factor in IQ test performance and outcomes. Therefore, results in the United States and Europe do not necessarily correlate to results in other populations.<ref>{{harvnb|Niu|Brass|2011}}</ref>

===Global variation of IQ scores===
{{further|Nations and intelligence}}
A number of studies have compared average IQ scores between the world's nations, finding patterns of difference between continental populations similar to those associated with race. ] and ] have argued that populations in the third world, particularly populations in Africa, tend to have limited intelligence because of their genetic composition and that, consequently, education cannot be effective in creating social and economic development in third world countries.{{sfn|Hunt|2010|p=437-439}} Lynn and Vanhanen's studies have been severely criticized for relying on low quality data and for choosing sources in ways that seem to be biased severely towards underestimating the average IQ potential of developing nations, particularly in Africa.{{sfn|Hunt|2010|pages=426–445}}{{sfn|Wicherts|Dolan|van der Maas|2009|page=10}}<ref>{{Cite journal | journal = Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of Books | date = August 2004 | volume = 49 | issue = 4 | pages = 389–396 | author1 = Barnett, Susan M. | author2 = Williams, Wendy | lastauthoramp = yes | title = National Intelligence and the Emperor's New Clothes | url = http://psycnet.apa.org/record/2004-17780-001 | doi = 10.1037/004367 | quote = we see an edifice built on layer upon layer of arbitrary assumptions and selective data manipulation. The data on which the entire book is based are of questionable validity and are used in ways that cannot be justified. }}</ref> Nonetheless there is a general consensus that the average IQ in developing countries is lower than in developed countries, but subsequent research has favored environmental explanations for this fact, such as lack of basic infrastructure related to health and education.

In the 2002 book '']'', and '']'' in 2006, Richard Lynn and Tatu Vanhanen created estimates of average IQs for 113 nations. They estimated IQs of 79 other nations based on neighboring nations or via other means. They saw a consistent correlation between national development and national IQ averages. They found the highest national IQs among ] and ]n developed nations and the lowest national IQs in the world's least developed nations among the indigenous peoples in the regions of Sub-Saharan Africa, Southeast Asia and Latin America.<ref>{{Cite book |title=The Global Bell Curve |last=Lynn |first=Richard |publisher=Washington Summit Publishers |year=2008 |pages=294–297}}</ref> In a meta-analysis of studies of IQ estimates in Sub-Saharan Africa, {{harvtxt|Wicherts|Dolan|van der Maas|2009|page=10}} concluded that Lynn and Vanhanen had relied on unsystematic methodology by failing to publish their criteria for including or excluding studies. They found that Lynn and Vanhanen's exclusion of studies had depressed their IQ estimate for sub-Saharan Africa, and that including studies excluded in "IQ and Global Inequality" resulted in average IQ of 82 for sub-Saharan Africa, lower than the average in Western countries, but higher than Lynn and Vanhanen's estimate of 67. Wicherts at al. conclude that this difference is likely due to sub-Saharan Africa having limited access to modern advances in education, nutrition and health care.{{sfn|Wicherts|Dolan|van der Maas|2009}}

A 2010 systematic review by the same research team, along with ], found that compared to American norms, the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans was about 80. The same review concluded that the ] had not yet taken hold in sub-Saharan Africa.{{sfn|Wicherts|Dolan|Carlson|van der Maas|2010}}

A 2007 meta-analysis by Rindermann found many of the same groupings and correlations found by Lynn and Vanhanen, with the lowest scores in sub-Saharan Africa, and a correlation of .60 between cognitive skill and GDP per capita. {{harvtxt|Hunt|2010|pp=437–439}} considers Rindermann's analysis to be much more reliable than Lynn and Vanhanen's. By measuring the relationship between educational data and social wellbeing over time, this study also performed a causal analysis, finding that when nations invest in education this leads to increased well-being later on.{{sfn|Hunt|2010|p=440-443}} Kamin (2006) has also criticized Lynn and Vanhanen's work on the IQs of sub-Saharan Africans.{{sfn|Kamin|2006}}


In response to the controversial 1994 book '']'', the ] (APA) formed a task-force of eleven experts, which issued a report "]" in 1996.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} Regarding group differences, the report reaffirmed the consensus that differences within groups are much wider than differences between groups, and that claims of ethnic differences in intelligence should be scrutinized carefully, as such claims had been used to justify racial discrimination. The report also acknowledged problems with the racial categories used, as these categories are neither consistently applied, nor homogeneous {{xref|(see ])}}.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}}
Wicherts, Borsboom & Dolan (2010) argue that studies reporting support for evolutionary theories of intelligence based on national IQ data suffer from multiple fatal methodological flaws. For example, they state that such studies "...assume that the Flynn Effect is either nonexistent or invariant with respect to different regions of the world, that there have been no migrations and climatic changes over the course of evolution, and that there have been no trends over the last century in indicators of reproductive strategies (e.g., declines in fertility and infant mortality)." They also showed that a strong degree of confounding exists between national IQs and current national development status.{{sfn|Wicherts|Borsboom|Dolan|2010}}


In the UK, some African groups have higher average educational attainment and standardized test scores than the overall population.<ref>{{cite journal |first1=Feyisa |last1=Demie |first2=Christabel |last2=McLean |title=Raising the achievement of African heritage pupils: a case study of good practice in British schools |journal=Educational Studies |date=1 December 2007 |issn=0305-5698 |pages=415–434 |volume=33 |issue=4 |doi=10.1080/03055690701423606 |s2cid=144579288}}</ref> In 2010–2011, white British pupils were 2.3% less likely to have gained 5 A*–C grades at ] than the national average, whereas the likelihood was 21.8% above average for those of ] origin, 5.5% above average for those of ] origin, and 1.4% above average for those of ] origin. For the two other African ethnic groups on which data was available, the likelihood was 23.7% below average for those of ] origin and 35.3% below average for those of ] origin.<ref>{{Cite book |last=Rutter |first=Jill |title=Back to basics: Towards a successful and cost-effective integration policy |work=IPPR |publisher=Institute for Public Policy Research |year=2013 |url=https://www.ippr.org/publications/back-to-basics-towards-a-successful-and-cost-effective-integration-policy |page=43 |access-date=2020-05-23 |archive-date=2020-04-13 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200413214928/https://www.ippr.org/publications/back-to-basics-towards-a-successful-and-cost-effective-integration-policy |url-status=live }}</ref> In 2014, Black-African pupils of 11 language groups were more likely to pass ] Maths 4+ in England than the national average. Overall, the average pass rate by ethnicity was 86.5% for white British (N = 395,787), whereas it was 85.6% for Black-Africans (N = 18,497). Nevertheless, several Black-African language groups, including ], ], ], ], ], ], ], ], ] speakers, and English-speaking Africans, each had an average pass rate above the white British average (total N = 9,314), with the Hausa, Igbo, Yoruba, and Amhara having averages above 90% (N = 2,071).<ref name=":0">{{Cite book |last=Feyisa Demie |first=Andrew Hau |title=Language Diversity and Attainment in Primary Schools in England |publisher=Lambeth Research And Statistics Unit |year=2016 |url=https://www.lambeth.gov.uk/rsu/sites/www.lambeth.gov.uk.rsu/files/language_diversity_and_attainment_in_primary_schools_in_england_2017.pdf |page=18 |access-date=2020-05-24 |archive-date=2020-08-06 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200806165229/https://www.lambeth.gov.uk/rsu/sites/www.lambeth.gov.uk.rsu/files/language_diversity_and_attainment_in_primary_schools_in_england_2017.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref> In 2017–2018, the percentage of pupils getting a strong pass (grade 5 or above) in the English and maths GCSE (in ]) was 42.7% for whites (N = 396,680) and 44.3% for Black-Africans (N = 18,358).<ref>{{Cite web |title=GCSE English and maths results |url=https://www.ethnicity-facts-figures.service.gov.uk/education-skills-and-training/11-to-16-years-old/a-to-c-in-english-and-maths-gcse-attainment-for-children-aged-14-to-16-key-stage-4/3.0 |date=2019 |website=Gov.UK |access-date=2022-09-20 |archive-date=2022-09-20 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20220920173733/https://www.ethnicity-facts-figures.service.gov.uk/education-skills-and-training/11-to-16-years-old/a-to-c-in-english-and-maths-gcse-attainment-for-children-aged-14-to-16-key-stage-4/3.0 |url-status=live }}</ref>
Similarly, Pesta & Poznanski (2014) showed that the average temperature of a given U.S. state is strongly associated with that state's average IQ and other well-being variables, despite the fact that evolution has not had enough time to operate on non-Native American residents of the United States. They also noted that this association persisted even after controlling for race, and concluded that "Evolution is therefore not necessary for temperature and IQ/well-being to co-vary meaningfully across geographic space."{{sfn|Pesta|Poznanski|2014}}


===Flynn effect and the closing gap=== === Flynn effect and the closing gap ===
{{Main|Flynn effect}} {{Main|Flynn effect}}
For the past century raw scores on IQ tests have been rising; this score increase is known as the "Flynn effect", named after ]. In the United States, the increase was continuous and approximately linear from the earliest years of testing to about 1998 when the gains stopped and some tests even showed decreasing test scores. For example, in the United States the average scores of blacks on some IQ tests in 1995 were the same as the scores of whites in 1945.<ref>{{harvnb|Mackintosh|1998|p=162}}</ref> As one pair of academics phrased it, "the typical African American today probably has a slightly higher IQ than the grandparents of today's average white American."<ref>{{cite book | last = Swain | first = Carol | title = Contemporary voices of white nationalism in America | publisher = Cambridge University Press | location = Cambridge, UK New York | year = 2003 | isbn = 978-0521016933 | page = 70}} Note: this quote is from the authors' introductory essay, not from the interviews.</ref> The ']' a term coined after researcher ] — refers to the substantial rise in raw IQ test scores observed in many parts of the world during the 20th century. In the United States, the increase was continuous and approximately linear from the earliest years of testing to about 1998 when the gains stopped and some tests even showed decreasing test scores. For example, the average scores of black people on some IQ tests in 1995 were the same as the scores of white people in 1945.<ref>{{harvnb|Mackintosh|1998|p=162}}</ref> As one pair of academics phrased it, "the typical African American today probably has a slightly higher IQ than the grandparents of today's average white American."<ref>{{cite book |last=Swain |first=Carol |title=Contemporary voices of white nationalism in America |publisher=Cambridge University Press |location=Cambridge, UK New York |year=2003 |isbn=978-0-521-01693-3 |page= |url=https://archive.org/details/contemporaryvoic00swai/page/70}} Note: this quote is from the authors' introductory essay, not from the interviews.</ref>


Flynn himself argued that the dramatic changes having taken place between one just generation and the next pointed strongly at an environmental explanation, and that it is highly unlikely that genetic factors could have accounted for the increasing scores. The Flynn effect, along with Flynn's analysis, continues to hold significance in the context of the black/white IQ gap debate, demonstrating the potential for environmental factors to influence IQ test scores by as much as 1 standard deviation, a scale of change that had previously been doubted.{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2001}}
Flynn has argued that given that these changes take place between one generation and the next it is highly unlikely that genetic factors could account for the increasing scores, which must then be caused by environmental factors. The Flynn Effect has often been used as an argument that the racial gap in IQ test scores must be environmental too, but this is not generally agreed – others have asserted that the two may have entirely different causes. A meta-analysis by Te Nijenhuis and ] (2013) concluded that the Flynn effect and group differences in intelligence were likely to have different causes. They stated that the Flynn effect is caused primarily by environmental factors and that it's unlikely these same environmental factors play an important role in explaining group differences in IQ.<ref name="Nijenhuis">{{cite journal|last1=te Nijenhuis|first1=Jan |last2=van der Flier |first2=Henk | author-link2 = Henk van der Flier | title=Is the Flynn effect on g?: A meta-analysis|journal=Intelligence|volume=41 |issue=6 |pages=802–807 |year=2013|url=|ref=harv |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.001}}</ref> The importance of the Flynn effect in the debate over the causes for the IQ gap lies in demonstrating that environmental factors may cause changes in test scores on the scale of 1 SD. This had previously been doubted.


A separate phenomenon from the Flynn effect has been the discovery that the IQ gap has been gradually closing over the last decades of the 20th century, as black test-takers increased their average scores relative to white test-takers. For instance, Vincent reported in 1991 that the black-white IQ gap was decreasing among children, but that it was remaining constant among adults.{{sfn|Vincent|1991}} Similarly, a 2006 study by Dickens and Flynn estimated that the difference between mean scores of blacks and whites closed by about 5 or 6 IQ points between 1972 and 2002,{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}} a reduction of about one-third. In the same period, the educational achievement disparity also diminished.<ref>Neisser, Ulric (Ed). 1998. The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association</ref> However, this was challenged by Rushton & Jensen who claim the difference remains stable.{{sfn|Rushton|Jensen|2006}} In a 2006 study, Murray agreed with Dickens and Flynn that there has been a narrowing of the difference; "Dickens' and Flynn's estimate of 3–6 IQ points from a base of about 16–18 points is a useful, though provisional, starting point". But he argued that this has stalled and that there has been no further narrowing for people born after the late 1970s.{{sfn|Murray|2006}} A subsequent study by Murray, based on the ], estimated that the black-white IQ difference decreased by about one-half of one standard deviation from those born in the 1920s to those born in the second half of the 1960s and early 1970s.{{sfn|Murray|2007}} Recent reviews by Flynn and Dickens (2006), Mackintosh (2011), and Nisbett ''et al.'' 2012 accept the gradual closing of the gap as a fact. In his review of the historical trends, {{harvtxt|Hunt|2010|page=411}} states: "There is some variety in the results, but not a great deal. The African American means are about 1 standard deviation unit (15 points on the IQ scale) below the white means, and the Hispanic means fall in between." A distinct but related observation has been the gradual narrowing of the American black-white IQ gap in the last decades of the 20th century, as black test-takers increased their average scores relative to white test-takers. For instance, Vincent reported in 1991 that the black–white IQ gap was decreasing among children, but that it was remaining constant among adults.{{sfn|Vincent|1991}} Similarly, a 2006 study by Dickens and Flynn estimated that the difference between mean scores of black people and white people closed by about 5 or 6 IQ points between 1972 and 2002,{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}} a reduction of about one-third. In the same period, the educational achievement disparity also diminished.<ref>Neisser, Ulric (Ed). 1998. The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association</ref> Reviews by Flynn and Dickens,{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}} Mackintosh,{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011}} and Nisbett ''et al.'' accept the gradual closing of the gap as a fact.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}} Flynn and Dickens summarize this trend, stating, "The constancy of the Black-White IQ gap is a myth and therefore cannot be cited as evidence that the racial IQ gap is genetic in origin."{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}}

Some studies reviewed by {{harvtxt|Hunt|2010|page=418}} found that rise in the average achievement of African Americans was caused by a reduction in the number of African American students in the lowest range of scores without a corresponding increase in the number of students in the highest ranges. A 2012 review of the literature found that the IQ gap had diminished by 0.33 standard deviations since first reported.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012}}{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012b}}

A 2013 analysis of the ] found that from 1971 to 2008, the size of the black–white IQ gap in the United States decreased from 16.33 to 9.94 IQ points. It has also concluded however that, while IQ means are continuing to rise in all ethnic groups, this growth is occurring more slowly among 17-year-old students than among younger students and the black-white IQ gap is no longer narrowing. As of 2008, a study published in 2013 by Heiner Rindermann, Stefan Pinchelmann, and James Thompson have estimated the IQ means of 17-year-old black, white, and Hispanic students to range respectively from 90.45–94.15, 102.29–104.57 and 92.30–95.90 points. They explain that the gap may persist due to the ], the degradation of ], the rise of fraud in the educational system (especially with respect to '']''), the decrease in unskilled real wages and employment among African-Americans due to ] and ] increases, differences in parental practices (such as breastfeeding or reading to children), and "environmental conditions shaped by themselves." To resolve this, they ultimately recommend the reestablishment of "meritoric principles" and "blindly graded objective central exams," as opposed to "]," in education.{{sfn|Rindermann|Thompson|2013}}<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Rindermann|first1=Heiner|last2=Pinchelmann|first2=Stefan|title=Future Cognitive Ability: US IQ Prediction until 2060 Based on NAEP|journal=Pone|date=13 October 2015|doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0138412|pmid=26460731|pmc=4603674|volume=10|issue=10|page=e0138412|bibcode=2015PLoSO..1038412R}}</ref>

==Environmental influences on group differences in IQ==
The following environmental factors are some of those suggested as explaining a portion of the differences in average IQ between races. These factors are not mutually exclusive with one another, and some may, in fact, contribute directly to others. Furthermore, the relationship between genetics and environmental factors may be complicated. For example, the differences in socioeconomic environment for a child may be due to differences in genetic IQ for the parents, and the differences in average brain size between races could be the result of nutritional factors.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} All recent reviews agree that some environmental factors that are unequally distributed between racial groups have been shown to affect intelligence in ways that could contribute to the test score gap. However, currently, the question is whether these factors can account for the entire gap between white and black test scores, or only part of it. One group of scholars, including ], ], Joshua Aronson, Diane Halpern, William Dickens, Eric Turkheimer (2012) have argued that the environmental factors so far demonstrated are sufficient to account for the entire gap. ] (2011) considers this a reasonable argument, but argues that probably it is impossible to ever know for sure; another group including ] (2010), ],{{sfn|Rushton|Jensen|2005}} ] and ] have argued that this is impossible. Jensen and Rushton consider that it may account for as little as 20% of the gap. Meanwhile, while Hunt considers this a vast overstatement, he nonetheless considers it likely that some portion of the gap will eventually be shown to be caused by genetic factors.


==Environmental factors==
===Health and nutrition=== ===Health and nutrition===
{{Main|Impact of health on intelligence}} {{Main|Impact of health on intelligence}}
] ]


Environmental factors including ],<ref name="Bellinger, Stiles & Needleman 1992"/> low rates of ],<ref name="Campbell et al. 2002">{{harvnb|Campbell et al.|2002}}</ref> and poor ]<ref>{{harvnb|Ivanovic et al.|2004}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Saloojee| Pettifor|2001}}</ref> can significantly affect cognitive development and functioning. For example, childhood exposure to lead, associated with homes in poorer areas<ref>{{Cite web|url=https://www.atsdr.cdc.gov/csem/ped_env_health/docs/ped_env_health.pdf|title=Principles of Pediatric Environmental Health, The Child as Susceptible Host: A Developmental Approach to Pediatric Environmental Medicine|last=Agency For Toxic Substances And Disease Registry Case Studies In Environmental Medicine (CSEM)|date=2012-02-15|website=U.S. Department for Health and Human Services|access-date=2019-01-30}}</ref> causes an average IQ drop of 7 points,<ref name="Lanphear Hornung Khoury Yolton 2005 pp. 894–899">{{cite journal | last=Lanphear | first=Bruce P. | last2=Hornung | first2=Richard | last3=Khoury | first3=Jane | last4=Yolton | first4=Kimberly | last5=Baghurst | first5=Peter | last6=Bellinger | first6=David C. | last7=Canfield | first7=Richard L. | last8=Dietrich | first8=Kim N. | last9=Bornschein | first9=Robert | last10=Greene | first10=Tom | last11=Rothenberg | first11=Stephen J. | last12=Needleman | first12=Herbert L. | last13=Schnaas | first13=Lourdes | last14=Wasserman | first14=Gail | last15=Graziano | first15=Joseph | last16=Roberts | first16=Russell | title=Low-Level Environmental Lead Exposure and Children's Intellectual Function: An International Pooled Analysis | journal=Environmental Health Perspectives | volume=113 | issue=7 | date=2005-03-18 | issn=0091-6765 | pmid=16002379 | pmc=1257652 | doi=10.1289/ehp.7688 | pages=894–899}}</ref> and ], on average, of 12 IQ points.<ref>{{harvnb|Qian et al.|2005}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |first=James |last=Feyrer |first2=Dimitra |last2=Politi |first3=David N. |last3=Weil |title=The Cognitive Effects of Micronutrient Deficiency: Evidence from Salt Iodization in the United States |year=2017 |journal=Journal of the European Economic Association |volume=15 |issue=2 |pages=355–387 |doi=10.1093/jeea/jvw002 |url=http://www.nber.org/papers/w19233.pdf }}</ref> Such impairments may sometimes be permanent, sometimes be partially or wholly compensated for by later growth. The first two years of life is the critical time for malnutrition, the consequences of which are often irreversible and include poor cognitive development, educability, and future economic productivity.<ref>, 2008.</ref><!--Which paper? Link points only to to listed series of papers (yes, need more specific, preferably secondary, reference) --> The African American population of the United States is statistically more likely to be exposed to many detrimental environmental factors such as poorer neighborhoods, schools, nutrition, and prenatal and postnatal health care.<ref>{{harvnb|Nisbett|2009|p=101}}</ref><ref name="Cooper 2005"/> Mackintosh points out that for American blacks infant mortality is about twice as high as for whites, and low birthweight is twice as prevalent. At the same time white mothers are twice as likely to breastfeed their infants, and breastfeeding is highly correlated with IQ for low birthweight infants. In this way a wide number of health related factors that influence IQ are unequally distributed between the two groups.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|pages=343–44}} Environmental factors including ],<ref name="Bellinger, Stiles & Needleman 1992"/> low rates of ],<ref name="Campbell et al. 2002">{{harvnb|Campbell et al.|2002}}</ref> and poor ]<ref>{{harvnb|Ivanovic et al.|2004}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Saloojee| Pettifor|2001}}</ref> are significantly correlated with poor cognitive development and functioning. For example, childhood exposure to {{nowrap|lead{{tsp}}{{mdash}}{{tsp}}}}associated with homes in poorer {{nowrap|areas<ref>{{Cite web |url=https://www.atsdr.cdc.gov/csem/ped_env_health/docs/ped_env_health.pdf |title=Principles of Pediatric Environmental Health, The Child as Susceptible Host: A Developmental Approach to Pediatric Environmental Medicine |last=Agency For Toxic Substances And Disease Registry Case Studies In Environmental Medicine (CSEM) |date=2012-02-15 |website=U.S. Department for Health and Human Services |access-date=2019-01-30 |archive-date=2019-01-31 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190131093309/https://www.atsdr.cdc.gov/csem/ped_env_health/docs/ped_env_health.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref>{{hsp}}{{mdash}}{{tsp}}}}correlates with an average IQ drop of 7 points,<ref name="Lanphear Hornung Khoury Yolton 2005 pp. 894–899">{{cite journal |last1=Lanphear |first1=Bruce P. |last2=Hornung |first2=Richard |last3=Khoury |first3=Jane |last4=Yolton |first4=Kimberly |last5=Baghurst |first5=Peter |last6=Bellinger |first6=David C. |last7=Canfield |first7=Richard L. |last8=Dietrich |first8=Kim N. |last9=Bornschein |first9=Robert |last10=Greene |first10=Tom |last11=Rothenberg |first11=Stephen J. |last12=Needleman |first12=Herbert L. |last13=Schnaas |first13=Lourdes |last14=Wasserman |first14=Gail |last15=Graziano |first15=Joseph |last16=Roberts |first16=Russell |title=Low-Level Environmental Lead Exposure and Children's Intellectual Function: An International Pooled Analysis |journal=Environmental Health Perspectives |volume=113 |issue=7 |date=2005-03-18 |issn=0091-6765 |pmid=16002379 |pmc=1257652 |doi=10.1289/ehp.7688 |pages=894–899|bibcode=2005EnvHP.113..894L }}</ref> and ], on average, of 12 IQ points.<ref>{{harvnb|Qian et al.|2005}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |first1=James |last1=Feyrer |first2=Dimitra |last2=Politi |first3=David N. |last3=Weil |title=The Cognitive Effects of Micronutrient Deficiency: Evidence from Salt Iodization in the United States |year=2017 |journal=Journal of the European Economic Association |volume=15 |issue=2 |pages=355–387 |doi=10.1093/jeea/jvw002 |pmid=31853231 |pmc=6919660 |url=http://www.nber.org/papers/w19233.pdf |access-date=2019-07-22 |archive-date=2020-08-13 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200813174601/https://www.nber.org/papers/w19233.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref> Such impairments may sometimes be permanent, but in some cases they be partially or wholly compensated for by later growth.
The first two years of life are critical for malnutrition, the consequences of which are often irreversible and include poor cognitive development, educability, and future economic productivity.<ref> {{Webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110717005704/http://www.thelancet.com/series/maternal-and-child-undernutrition |date=2011-07-17 }}, 2008.</ref><!--Which paper? Link points only to to listed series of papers (yes, need more specific, preferably secondary, reference) --> Mackintosh points out that, for American black people, infant mortality is about twice as high as for white people, and low birth weight is twice as prevalent. At the same time, white mothers are twice as likely to breastfeed their infants, and breastfeeding is directly correlated with IQ for low-birth-weight infants. In this way, a wide number of health-related factors which influence IQ are unequally distributed between the two groups.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|pages=343–44}}


The ] in 2004 stated that lack of both iodine and iron has been implicated in impaired brain development, and this can affect enormous numbers of people: it is estimated that one-third of the total global population are affected by ]. In developing countries, it is estimated that 40% of children aged four and under suffer from ] because of insufficient iron in their diets.<ref>{{harvnb|Behrman|Alderman|Hoddinott|2004}}</ref> The ] in 2004 stated that lack of both iodine and iron has been implicated in impaired brain development, and this can affect enormous numbers of people: it is estimated that one-third of the total global population is affected by ]. In developing countries, it is estimated that 40% of children aged four and under have ] because of insufficient iron in their diets.<ref>{{harvnb|Behrman|Alderman|Hoddinott|2004}}</ref>


Other scholars have found that simply the standard of nutrition has a significant effect on population intelligence, and that the Flynn effect may be caused by increasing nutrition standards across the world.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Colom | first1 = R. | last2 = Lluis-Font | first2 = J. M. | last3 = Andrés-Pueyo | first3 = A. | year = 2005 | title = The generational intelligence gains are caused by decreasing variance in the lower half of the distribution: supporting evidence for the nutrition hypothesis | url = | journal = Intelligence | volume = 33 | issue = | pages = 83–91 | doi=10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.010}}</ref> James Flynn has himself argued against this view.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Flynn | first1 = J. R. | year = 2009 | title = Requiem for nutrition as the cause of IQ gains: Raven's gains in Britain 1938 to 2008 | url = | journal = Economics and Human Biology | volume = 7 | issue = 1| pages = 18–27 | doi=10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.009| pmid = 19251490 }}</ref> Other scholars have found that simply the standard of nutrition has a significant effect on population intelligence, and that the Flynn effect may be caused by increasing nutrition standards across the world.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Colom |first1=R. |last2=Lluis-Font |first2=J. M. |last3=Andrés-Pueyo |first3=A. |year=2005 |title=The generational intelligence gains are caused by decreasing variance in the lower half of the distribution: supporting evidence for the nutrition hypothesis |journal=Intelligence |volume=33 |pages=83–91 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.010}}</ref> James Flynn has himself argued against this view.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Flynn |first1=J. R. |year=2009a |title=Requiem for nutrition as the cause of IQ gains: Raven's gains in Britain 1938 to 2008 |journal=Economics and Human Biology |volume=7 |issue=1 |pages=18–27 |doi=10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.009 |pmid=19251490}}</ref>


Some recent research has argued that the retardation caused in brain development by ]s, many of which are more prevalent in non-white populations, may be an important factor in explaining the differences in IQ between different regions of the world.<ref>{{harvnb|Eppig|Fincher|Thornhill|2010}}</ref> The findings of this research, showing the correlation between IQ, race and infectious diseases was also shown to apply to the IQ gap in the US, suggesting that this may be an important environmental factor.<ref name="Eppig 2011">{{harvnb|Eppig|2011}}</ref> Some recent research has argued that the retardation caused in brain development by ]s, many of which are more prevalent in non-white populations, may be an important factor in explaining the differences in IQ between different regions of the world. The findings of this research, showing the correlation between IQ, race and infectious diseases was also shown to apply to the IQ gap in the US, suggesting that this may be an important environmental factor.<ref name="Eppig 2011">{{harvnb|Eppig|2011}}</ref>


A 2013 meta-analysis by the World Health Organization found that, after controlling for maternal IQ, breastfeeding was associated with IQ gains of 2.19 points. The authors suggest that this relationship is causal but state that the practical significance of this gain is debatable; however, they highlight one study suggesting an association between breastfeeding and academic performance in Brazil, where "breastfeeding duration does not present marked variability by socioeconomic position."<ref>{{Cite web|title=Long-term effects of breastfeeding – a systemic review|authors=Bernardo L. Horta and Cesar G. Victoria|publisher=World Health Organization|year=2013|accessdate=18 June 2018|url=http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/79198/9789241505307_eng.pdf}}</ref> Colen and Ramey (2014) similarly find that controlling for sibling comparisons within families, rather than between families, reduces the correlation between breastfeeding status and WISC IQ scores by nearly a third, but further find the relationship between breastfeeding duration and WISC IQ scores to be insignificant. They suggest that "much of the beneficial long-term effects typically attributed to breastfeeding, per se, may primarily be due to selection pressures into infant feeding practices along key demographic characteristics such as race and socioeconomic status."<ref>{{cite journal |author=Cynthia G. Colen and David M. Ramey |journal=Social Science & Medicine |volume=109 |issue=1 |pages=55–65 |year=2014 |pmc=4077166 |title=Is Breast Truly Best? Estimating the Effect of Breastfeeding on Long-term Child Wellbeing in the United States Using Sibling Comparisons |doi=10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.01.027|pmid=24698713 }}</ref> Reichman estimates that no more than 3 to 4% of the black-white IQ gap can be explained by black-white disparities in low birth weight.<ref>{{harvnb|Reichman|2005}}</ref> A 2013 meta-analysis by the World Health Organization found that, after controlling for maternal IQ, breastfeeding was associated with IQ gains of 2.19 points. The authors suggest that this relationship is causal but state that the practical significance of this gain is debatable; however, they highlight one study suggesting an association between breastfeeding and academic performance in Brazil, where "breastfeeding duration does not present marked variability by socioeconomic position."<ref>{{Cite web |title=Long-term effects of breastfeeding – a systemic review |first1=Bernardo L. |last1=Horta |first2=Cesar G. |last2=Victoria |publisher=World Health Organization |year=2013 |access-date=18 June 2018 |url=http://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/79198/9789241505307_eng.pdf |archive-date=9 April 2020 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200409233115/https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/79198/9789241505307_eng.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref> Colen and Ramey (2014) similarly find that controlling for sibling comparisons within families, rather than between families, reduces the correlation between breastfeeding status and WISC IQ scores by nearly a third, but further find the relationship between breastfeeding duration and WISC IQ scores to be insignificant. They suggest that "much of the beneficial long-term effects typically attributed to breastfeeding, per se, may primarily be due to selection pressures into infant feeding practices along key demographic characteristics such as race and socioeconomic status."<ref>{{cite journal |first1=Cynthia G. |last1=Colen |first2=David M. |last2=Ramey |journal=Social Science & Medicine |volume=109 |issue=1 |pages=55–65 |year=2014 |pmc=4077166 |title=Is Breast Truly Best? Estimating the Effect of Breastfeeding on Long-term Child Wellbeing in the United States Using Sibling Comparisons |doi=10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.01.027 |pmid=24698713}}</ref> Reichman estimates that no more than 3 to 4% of the black–white IQ gap can be explained by black–white disparities in low birth weight.<ref>{{harvnb|Reichman|2005}}</ref>


===Education=== ===Education===
Several studies have proposed that a large part of the gap can be attributed to differences in quality of education.<ref>{{harvnb|Manly et al.|2002}} and {{harvnb|Manly et al.|2004}}</ref> ] in education has been proposed as one possible cause of differences in educational quality between races.<ref>{{harvnb|Mickelson|2003}}</ref> According to a paper by Hala Elhoweris, Kagendo Mutua, Negmeldin Alsheikh and Pauline Holloway, teachers' referral decisions for students to participate in ] educational programs were influenced in part by the students' ethnicity.<ref>{{harvnb|Elhoweris et al.|2005}}</ref> Several studies have proposed that a large part of the gap in IQ test performance can be attributed to differences in quality of education.<ref>{{harvnb|Manly et al.|2002}} and {{harvnb|Manly et al.|2004}}</ref> ] in education has been proposed as one possible cause of differences in educational quality between races.<ref>{{harvnb|Mickelson|2003}}</ref> According to a paper by Hala Elhoweris, Kagendo Mutua, Negmeldin Alsheikh and Pauline Holloway, teachers' referral decisions for students to participate in ] educational programs were influenced in part by the students' ethnicity.<ref>{{harvnb|Elhoweris et al.|2005}}</ref>


The ], an intensive early childhood education project, was also able to bring about an average IQ gain of 4.4 points at age 21 in the black children who participated in it compared to controls.<ref name="Campbell et al. 2002"/> ] agreed that the Abecedarian project demonstrates that education can have a significant effect on IQ, but also said that no educational program thus far has been able to reduce the black-white IQ gap by more than a third, and that differences in education are thus unlikely to be its only cause.<ref>{{harvnb|Miele|2002|p=133}}</ref> The ], an intensive early childhood education project, was also able to bring about an average IQ gain of 4.4 points at age 21 in the black children who participated in it compared to controls.<ref name="Campbell et al. 2002"/> ] agreed that the Abecedarian project demonstrated that education can have a significant effect on IQ, but also declared his view that no educational program thus far had been able to reduce the black–white IQ gap by more than a third, and that differences in education are thus unlikely to be its only cause.<ref>{{harvnb|Miele|2002|p=133}}</ref>


A series of studies by ] and Cynthia Holland measured the effect of prior exposure to the kind of cognitive tasks posed in IQ tests on test performance. Assuming that the IQ gap was the result of lower exposure to tasks using the cognitive functions usually found in IQ tests among African American test takers, they prepared a group of African Americans in this type of tasks before taking an IQ test. The researchers found that there was no subsequent difference in performance between the African-Americans and white test takers.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Fagan |first1=Joseph F |last2=Holland |first2=Cynthia R |year=2002 |title=Equal opportunity and racial differences in IQ |journal=Intelligence |volume=30 |issue=4 |pages=361–387 |doi=10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00080-6}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Fagan |first1=J.F. |last2=Holland |first2=C.R. |year=2007 |title=Racial equality in intelligence: Predictions from a theory of intelligence as processing |journal=Intelligence |volume=35 |issue=4 |pages=319–334 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.08.009}}</ref> Daley and Onwuegbuzie conclude that Fagan and Holland demonstrate that "differences in knowledge between black people and white people for intelligence test items can be erased when equal opportunity is provided for exposure to the information to be tested".{{sfn|Daley|Onwuegbuzie|2011}} A similar argument is made by ] who argues that IQ differences correlate well with differences in literacy suggesting that developing literacy skills through education causes an increase in IQ test performance.<ref name="Marks, D.F. 2010">{{cite journal |last1=Marks |first1=D.F. |year=2010 |title=IQ variations across time, race, and nationality: An artifact of differences in literacy skills |journal=Psychological Reports |volume=106 |issue=3 |pages=643–664 |doi=10.2466/pr0.106.3.643-664 |pmid=20712152 |s2cid=12179547}}</ref><ref name="psychologytoday.com">{{cite magazine |last=Barry |first=Scott |url=http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/beautiful-minds/201008/the-flynn-effect-and-iq-disparities-among-races-ethnicities-and-nations- |title=The Flynn Effect and IQ Disparities Among Races, Ethnicities, and Nations: Are There Common Links? |magazine=Psychology Today |date=2010-08-23 |access-date=2014-08-22 |archive-date=2023-03-20 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230320043730/https://www.psychologytoday.com/intl/blog/beautiful-minds/201008/the-flynn-effect-and-iq-disparities-among-races-ethnicities-and-nations |url-status=live }}</ref>
Rushton and Jensen argue that long-term follow-up of the ] found large immediate gains for blacks and whites but that these were quickly lost for the blacks although some remained for whites. They argue that also other more intensive and prolonged educational interventions have not produced lasting effects on IQ or scholastic performance.<ref name="Rushton & Jensen 2005">{{harvnb|Rushton|Jensen|2005}}</ref> Nisbett argues that they ignore studies such as {{harvp|Campbell|Ramey|1994}} which found that at the age 12, 87% of black infants exposed to an intervention had IQs in the normal range (above 85) compared to 56% of controls, and none of the intervention-exposed children were mildly retarded compared to 7% of controls. Other early intervention programs have shown IQ effects in the range of 4–5 points, which are sustained until at least age 8–15. Effects on academic achievement can also be substantial. Nisbett also argues that not only early age intervention can be effective, citing other successful intervention studies from infancy to college.<ref>{{harvnb|Nisbett|2005|pp=303–4}}</ref>


A 2003 study found that two variables—] and the degree of educational attainment of children's fathers—partially explained the black–white gap in cognitive ability test scores, undermining the hereditarian view that they stemmed from immutable genetic factors.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=McKay |first1=Patrick F. |last2=Doverspike |first2=Dennis |last3=Bowen-Hilton |first3=Doreen |last4=McKay |first4=Quintonia D. |title=The Effects of Demographic Variables and Stereotype Threat on Black/White Differences in Cognitive Ability Test Performance |journal=Journal of Business and Psychology |date=2003 |volume=18 |issue=1 |pages=1–14 |doi=10.1023/A:1025062703113 |s2cid=142317051}}</ref>
A series of studies by ] and Cynthia Holland measured the effect of prior exposure to the kind of cognitive tasks posed in IQ tests on test performance. Assuming that the IQ gap was the result of lower exposure to tasks using the cognitive functions usually found in IQ tests among African American test takes, they prepared a group of African Americans in this type of tasks before taking an IQ test. The researchers found that there was no subsequent difference in performance between the African-Americans and white test takers.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Fagan | first1 = Joseph F | last2 = Holland | first2 = Cynthia R | year = 2002 | title = Equal opportunity and racial differences in IQ | url = | journal = Intelligence | volume = 30 | issue = 4| pages = 361–387 | doi = 10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00080-6 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Fagan | first1 = J.F. | last2 = Holland | first2 = C.R. | year = 2007 | title = Racial equality in intelligence: Predictions from a theory of intelligence as processing | url = | journal = Intelligence | volume = 35 | issue = 4| pages = 319–334 | doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.08.009}}</ref> Daley and Onwuegbuzie conclude that Fagan and Holland demonstrate that "differences in knowledge between blacks and whites for intelligence test items can be erased when equal opportunity is provided for exposure to the information to be tested".{{sfn|Daley|Onwuegbuzie|2011}} A similar argument is made by ] who argues that IQ differences correlate well with differences in literacy suggesting that developing literacy skills through education causes an increase in IQ test performance.<ref name="Marks, D.F. 2010">{{cite journal | last1 = Marks | first1 = D.F. | year = 2010 | title = IQ variations across time, race, and nationality: An artifact of differences in literacy skills | url = | journal = Psychological Reports | volume = 106 | issue = 3| pages = 643–664 | doi=10.2466/pr0.106.3.643-664 | pmid=20712152}}</ref><ref name="psychologytoday.com">{{cite magazine|last=Barry |first=Scott |url=http://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/beautiful-minds/201008/the-flynn-effect-and-iq-disparities-among-races-ethnicities-and-nations- |title=The Flynn Effect and IQ Disparities Among Races, Ethnicities, and Nations: Are There Common Links? |magazine=Psychology Today |date=2010-08-23 |accessdate=2014-08-22}}</ref>

A 2003 study found that two variables — stereotype threat and the degree of educational attainment of children's fathers — partially explained the black-white gap in cognitive ability test scores, undermining the hereditarian view that they stemmed from immutable genetic factors.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=McKay|first1=Patrick F.|last2=Doverspike|first2=Dennis|last3=Bowen‐Hilton|first3=Doreen|last4=McKay|first4=Quintonia D.|title=The Effects of Demographic Variables and Stereotype Threat on Black/White Differences in Cognitive Ability Test Performance|journal=Journal of Business and Psychology|date=2003|volume=18|issue=1|pages=1–14|doi=10.1023/A:1025062703113}}</ref>


===Socioeconomic environment=== ===Socioeconomic environment===
Different aspects of the socioeconomic environment in which children are raised have been shown to correlate with part of the IQ gap, but they do not account for the entire gap.{{sfn|Hunt|2010|page=428}} According to a 2006 review, these factors account for slightly less than half of one standard deviation of the gap.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Magnuson|first1=Katherine A.|last2=Duncan|first2=Greg J.|title=The role of family socioeconomic resources in the black–white test score gap among young children|journal=]|date=December 2006|volume=26|issue=4|pages=365–399|doi=10.1016/j.dr.2006.06.004}}</ref> Generally the difference between mean test scores of blacks and whites is not eliminated when individuals and groups are matched on socioeconomic status (SES), suggesting that the relationship between IQ and SES is not simply one in which SES determines IQ. Rather it may be the case that differences in intelligence, particularly parental intelligence, may also cause differences in SES, making separating the two factors difficult.<ref name="Neisser et al. 1996">{{harvnb|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}}</ref> {{harvtxt|Hunt|2010|page=428}} summarises data {{clarify|date = December 2015|reason = which research?}} showing that, jointly, SES and parental IQ account for the full gap (in populations of young children, after controlling parental IQ and parental SES, the gap is not statistically different from zero). He argues the SES-linked components reflect parental occupation status, mother's verbal comprehension score and parent-child interaction quality. Hunt also reviews data showing that the correlation between home environment and IQ becomes weaker with age.{{citation needed|date=December 2015}} Hart and Risley argue that in welfare, working-class, and professional families, children hear a large disparity in the amount of language (between 13 million and 45 million words) in the age range of 0–3, and that by age 9 these differences lead to large differences in child outcomes.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Hart|first1=Betty|last2=Risley|first2=Todd|title=Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children|publisher=Brookes Publishing, Baltimore, Maryland, USA|year=1995}}</ref> Different aspects of the socioeconomic environment in which children are raised have been shown to correlate with part of the IQ gap, but they do not account for the entire gap.{{sfn|Hunt|2010|page=428}} According to a 2006 review, these factors account for slightly less than half of one standard deviation.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Magnuson |first1=Katherine A. |last2=Duncan |first2=Greg J. |title=The role of family socioeconomic resources in the black–white test score gap among young children |journal=] |date=December 2006 |volume=26 |issue=4 |pages=365–399 |doi=10.1016/j.dr.2006.06.004}}</ref>


Other research has focussed on different causes of variation within low SES and high SES groups.<ref name="Scarr-Salapatek1971">{{cite journal | last1 = Scarr-Salapatek | first1 = S. | year = 1971 | title = Race, social class, and IQ. | url = | journal = Science | volume = 174 | issue = 4016| pages = 1285–95 | doi=10.1126/science.174.4016.1285| pmid = 5167501 | bibcode = 1971Sci...174.1285S}}</ref><ref name="Scarr-Salapatek1974">{{cite journal | last1 = Scarr-Salapatek | first1 = S. | year = 1974 | title = Some myths about heritability and IQ. | doi = 10.1038/251463b0 | journal = Nature | volume = 251 | issue = 5475| pages = 463–464 | bibcode = 1974Natur.251..463S }}</ref><ref name="Rowe1994">D. C. Rowe. (1994). ''The Limits of Family Influence: Genes, Experience and Behaviour''. Guilford Press. London</ref> Other research has focused on different causes of variation within low socioeconomic status (SES) and high SES groups.<ref name="Scarr-Salapatek1971">{{cite journal |last1=Scarr-Salapatek |first1=S. |year=1971 |title=Race, social class, and IQ. |journal=Science |volume=174 |issue=4016 |pages=1285–95 |doi=10.1126/science.174.4016.1285 |pmid=5167501 |bibcode=1971Sci...174.1285S}}</ref><ref name="Scarr-Salapatek1974">{{cite journal |last1=Scarr-Salapatek |first1=S. |year=1974 |title=Some myths about heritability and IQ. |doi=10.1038/251463b0 |journal=Nature |volume=251 |issue=5475 |pages=463–464 |bibcode=1974Natur.251..463S |s2cid=32437709 |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name="Rowe1994">D. C. Rowe. (1994). ''The Limits of Family Influence: Genes, Experience and Behaviour''. Guilford Press. London</ref>
In the US, among low-SES groups, genetic differences account for a smaller proportion variance in IQ than among higher SES populations.<ref name="Kirkpatrick2015">{{cite journal | last1 = Kirkpatrick | first1 = R. M. | last2 = McGue | first2 = M. | last3 = Iacono | first3 = W. G. | year = 2015 | title = Replication of a gene-environment interaction Via Multimodel inference: additive-genetic variance in adolescents' general cognitive ability increases with family-of-origin socioeconomic status | doi = 10.1007/s10519-014-9698-y | journal = Behav Genet | volume = 45 | issue = 2| pages = 200–14 | pmc = 4374354 | pmid=25539975}}</ref> Such effects are predicted by the '']'' hypothesis – that genotypes are transformed into phenotypes through nonadditive synergistic effects of the environment.<ref>{{Cite journal|title = Nature-nuture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model.|journal = Psychological Review|pages = 568–586|volume = 101|issue = 4|doi = 10.1037/0033-295x.101.4.568|first = Urie|last = Bronfenbrenner|first2 = Stephen J.|last2 = Ceci|pmid=7984707|date=October 1994}}</ref> {{harvp|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012}} suggest that high SES individuals are more likely to be able to develop their full biological potential, whereas low SES individuals are likely to be hindered in their development by adverse environmental conditions. The same review also points out that adoption studies generally are biased towards including only high and high middle SES adoptive families, meaning that they will tend to overestimate average genetic effects. They also note that studies of adoption from lower-class homes to middle-class homes have shown that such children experience a 12–18 pt gain in IQ relative to children who remain in low SES homes.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012}} A 2015 study found that environmental factors (namely, family income, maternal education, maternal verbal ability/knowledge, learning materials in the home, parenting factors (maternal sensitivity, maternal warmth and acceptance, and safe physical environment), child birth order, and child birth weight) accounted for the black-white gap in cognitive ability test scores.{{sfn|Cottrell|Newman|Roisman|2015}} In the US, among low SES groups, genetic differences account for a smaller proportion of the variance in IQ than among high SES populations.<ref name="Kirkpatrick2015">{{cite journal |last1=Kirkpatrick |first1=R. M. |last2=McGue |first2=M. |last3=Iacono |first3=W. G. |year=2015 |title=Replication of a gene-environment interaction Via Multimodel inference: additive-genetic variance in adolescents' general cognitive ability increases with family-of-origin socioeconomic status |doi=10.1007/s10519-014-9698-y |journal=Behav Genet |volume=45 |issue=2 |pages=200–14 |pmc=4374354 |pmid=25539975}}</ref> Such effects are predicted by the '']'' hypothesis—that genotypes are transformed into phenotypes through nonadditive synergistic effects of the environment.<ref>{{Cite journal |title=Nature-nuture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. |journal=Psychological Review |pages=568–586 |volume=101 |issue=4 |doi=10.1037/0033-295x.101.4.568 |first1=Urie |last1=Bronfenbrenner |first2=Stephen J. |last2=Ceci |pmid=7984707 |date=October 1994|s2cid=17402964 }}</ref> {{harvp|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}} suggest that high SES individuals are more likely to be able to develop their full biological potential, whereas low SES individuals are likely to be hindered in their development by adverse environmental conditions. The same review also points out that adoption studies generally are biased towards including only high and high middle SES adoptive families, meaning that they will tend to overestimate average genetic effects. They also note that studies of adoption from lower-class homes to middle-class homes have shown that such children experience a 12 to 18 point gain in IQ relative to children who remain in low SES homes.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}} A 2015 study found that environmental factors (namely, family income, maternal education, maternal verbal ability/knowledge, learning materials in the home, parenting factors, child birth order, and child birth weight) accounted for the black–white gap in cognitive ability test scores.{{sfn|Cottrell|Newman|Roisman|2015}}


===Test bias=== ===Test bias===
A number of studies have reached the conclusion that IQ tests may be biased against certain groups.<ref>{{harvnb|Cronshaw et al.|2006|p=278}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Verney et al.|2005}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Borsboom|2006}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Shuttleworth-Edwards et al.|2004}}</ref> The validity and reliability of IQ scores obtained from outside the United States and Europe have been questioned, in part because of the inherent difficulty of comparing IQ scores between cultures.<ref name="Richardson 2004">{{harvnb|Richardson|2004}}</ref><ref name="Hunt & Wittmann 2008">{{harvnb|Hunt|Wittmann|2008}}</ref> Several researchers have argued that cultural differences limit the appropriateness of standard IQ tests in non-industrialized communities.<ref>{{harvnb|Irvine|1983}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Irvine|Berry|1988}} a collection of articles by several authors discussing the limits of assessment by intelligence tests in different communities in the world. In particular, {{harvp|Reuning|1988}} describes the difficulties in devising and administering tests for Kalahari bushmen.</ref> A number of studies have reached the conclusion that IQ tests may be biased against certain groups.<ref>{{harvnb|Cronshaw et al.|2006|p=278}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Verney et al.|2005}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Borsboom|2006}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Shuttleworth-Edwards et al.|2004}}</ref> The validity and reliability of IQ scores obtained from outside the United States and Europe have been questioned, in part because of the inherent difficulty of comparing IQ scores between cultures.<ref name="Richardson 2004">{{harvnb|Richardson|2004}}</ref><ref name="Hunt & Wittmann 2008">{{harvnb|Hunt|Wittmann|2008}}</ref> Several researchers have argued that cultural differences limit the appropriateness of standard IQ tests in non-industrialized communities.<ref>{{harvnb|Irvine|1983}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Irvine|Berry|1988}} a collection of articles by several authors discussing the limits of assessment by intelligence tests in different communities in the world. In particular, {{harvp|Reuning|1988}} describes the difficulties in devising and administering tests for Kalahari bushmen.</ref>


A 1996 report by the ] states that intelligence can be difficult to compare across cultures, and notes that differing familiarity with test materials can produce substantial differences in test results; it also says that tests are accurate predictors of future achievement for black and white Americans, and are in that sense unbiased.<ref name="Neisser et al. 1996"/> The view that tests accurately predict future educational attainment is reinforced by ] in his 1998 book ''IQ and Human Intelligence'',<ref>{{harvnb|Mackintosh|1998|p=174}}: "Despite widespread belief to the contrary, however, there is ample evidence, both in Britain and the USA, that IQ tests predict educational attainment just about as well in ethnic minorities as in the white majority."</ref> and by a 1999 literature review by {{harvp|Brown|Reynolds|Whitaker|1999}}. A 1996 report by the ] states that intelligence can be difficult to compare across cultures, and notes that differing familiarity with test materials can produce substantial differences in test results; it also says that tests are accurate predictors of future achievement for black and white Americans, and are in that sense unbiased.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} The view that tests accurately predict future educational attainment is reinforced by ] in his 1998 book ''IQ and Human Intelligence'',<ref>{{harvnb|Mackintosh|1998|p=174}}: "Despite widespread belief to the contrary, however, there is ample evidence, both in Britain and the USA, that IQ tests predict educational attainment just about as well in ethnic minorities as in the white majority."</ref> and by a 1999 literature review by {{harvp|Brown|Reynolds|Whitaker|1999}}.


James R. Flynn, surveying studies on the topic, notes that the weight and presence of many test questions depends on what sorts of information and modes of thinking are culturally valued.<ref name="FlynnIntelligence">{{cite journal |journal=Intelligence |issue=70 |pages=73–83|year=2018|url=https://scottbarrykaufman.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/08/1-s2.0-S0160289618300904-main.pdf |title=Reflections about intelligence over 40 years}}</ref> James R. Flynn, surveying studies on the topic, notes that the weight and presence of many test questions depends on what sorts of information and modes of thinking are culturally valued.<ref name="FlynnIntelligence">{{cite journal |journal=Intelligence |issue=70 |pages=73–83 |year=2018 |url=https://scottbarrykaufman.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/08/1-s2.0-S0160289618300904-main.pdf |title=Reflections about intelligence over 40 years |access-date=2019-02-02 |archive-date=2019-02-03 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190203030438/https://scottbarrykaufman.com/wp-content/uploads/2018/08/1-s2.0-S0160289618300904-main.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref>


===Stereotype threat and minority status=== ===Stereotype threat and minority status===
{{Main|Stereotype threat}} {{Main|Stereotype threat}}
] is the fear that one's behavior will confirm an existing ] of a group with which one identifies or by which one is defined; this fear may in turn lead to an impairment of performance.<ref>{{harvnb|Aronson|Wilson|Akert| 2005}}</ref> Testing situations that highlight the fact that intelligence is being measured tend to lower the scores of individuals from racial-ethnic groups who already score lower on average or are expected to score lower. Stereotype threat conditions cause larger than expected IQ differences among groups.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Steele|first1=Claude M.|title=A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance|journal=American Psychologist|volume=52|issue=6|year=1997|pages=613–629|issn=0003-066X|doi=10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613|pmid=9174398|citeseerx=10.1.1.319.8283}}</ref> Psychometrician ] considers that there is little doubt that the effects of stereotype threat contribute to the IQ gap between blacks and whites.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|p=348}} ] is the fear that one's behavior will confirm an existing ] of a group with which one identifies or by which one is defined; this fear may in turn lead to an impairment of performance.<ref>{{harvnb|Aronson|Wilson|Akert| 2005}}</ref> Testing situations that highlight the fact that intelligence is being measured tend to lower the scores of individuals from racial-ethnic groups who already score lower on average or are expected to score lower. Stereotype threat conditions cause larger than expected IQ differences among groups.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Steele |first1=Claude M. |title=A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance |journal=American Psychologist |volume=52 |issue=6 |year=1997 |pages=613–629 |issn=0003-066X |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613 |pmid=9174398 |citeseerx=10.1.1.319.8283|s2cid=19952 }}</ref> Psychometrician ] considers that there is little doubt that the effects of stereotype threat contribute to the IQ gap between black people and white people.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|p=348}}


A large number of studies have shown that systemically disadvantaged minorities, such as the African American minority of the United States, generally perform worse in the educational system and in intelligence tests than the majority groups or less disadvantaged minorities such as immigrant or "voluntary" minorities.<ref name="Neisser et al. 1996"/> The explanation of these findings may be that children of caste-like minorities, due to the systemic limitations of their prospects of social advancement, do not have "]", i.e. they do not have the confidence that acquiring the skills valued by majority society, such as those skills measured by IQ tests, is worthwhile. They may even deliberately reject certain behaviors that are seen as "]."{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}}{{sfn|Ogbu|1978}}{{sfn|Ogbu|1994}} A large number of studies have shown that systemically disadvantaged minorities, such as the African American minority of the United States, generally perform worse in the educational system and in intelligence tests than the majority groups or less disadvantaged minorities such as immigrant or "voluntary" minorities.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} The explanation of these findings may be that children of caste-like minorities, due to the systemic limitations of their prospects of social advancement, do not have "]", i.e. they do not have the confidence that acquiring the skills valued by majority society, such as those skills measured by IQ tests, is worthwhile. They may even deliberately reject certain behaviors that are seen as "]."{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}}{{sfn|Ogbu|1978}}{{sfn|Ogbu|1994}} Research published in 1997 indicates that part of the black–white gap in cognitive ability test scores is due to racial differences in test motivation.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Chan |first1=D. |last2=Schmitt |first2=N. |last3=DeShon |first3=R. P. |last4=Clause |first4=C. S. |last5=Delbridge |first5=K. |date=April 1997 |title=Reactions to cognitive ability tests: the relationships between race, test performance, face validity perceptions, and test-taking motivation |journal=The Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=82 |issue=2 |pages=300–310 |issn=0021-9010 |pmid=9109288 |doi=10.1037/0021-9010.82.2.300|url=https://ink.library.smu.edu.sg/soss_research/230 }}</ref>


Some researchers have suggested that stereotype threat should not be interpreted as a factor in real-life performance gaps, and have raised the possibility of ].<ref name="Ganley2013">{{cite journal |vauthors=Ganley CM, Mingle LA, Ryan AM, Ryan K, Vasilyeva M, Perry M |title=An examination of stereotype threat effects on girls' mathematics performance |journal=Developmental Psychology |volume=49 |issue=10 |pages=1886–97 |date=October 2013 |pmid=23356523 |doi=10.1037/a0031412 |url=https://dl.dropboxusercontent.com/u/85192141/2013-ganley.pdf |citeseerx=10.1.1.353.4436 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20140719005546/https://dl.dropboxusercontent.com/u/85192141/2013-ganley.pdf |archive-date=19 July 2014}}</ref><ref name="Stoet2012">{{Cite journal |vauthors=Stoet G, Geary DC |doi=10.1037/a0026617 |title=Can stereotype threat explain the gender gap in mathematics performance and achievement? |journal=Review of General Psychology |volume=16 |pages=93–102 |year=2012 |s2cid=145724069}} {{webarchive|url=https://web.archive.org/web/20160112130459/http://volition.gla.ac.uk/~stoet/pdf/Stoet-Geary-RGP2012.pdf |date=2016-01-12 }}</ref><ref name="Flore2014">{{cite journal |vauthors=Flore PC, Wicherts JM |title=Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis |journal=Journal of School Psychology |volume=53 |issue=1 |pages=25–44 |date=February 2015 |pmid=25636259 |doi=10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002|s2cid=206516995 }}</ref> Other critics have focused on correcting what they claim are misconceptions of early studies showing a large effect.<ref name="Sackett2004a">{{cite journal |vauthors=Sackett PR, Hardison CM, Cullen MJ |title=On interpreting stereotype threat as accounting for African American-White differences on cognitive tests |journal=The American Psychologist |volume=59 |issue=1 |pages=7–13 |date=January 2004 |pmid=14736315 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.7 |url=http://www2.uni-jena.de/svw/igc/studies/ss03/sackitt_hardison_cullen_2004.pdf |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130404150510/http://www2.uni-jena.de/svw/igc/studies/ss03/sackitt_hardison_cullen_2004.pdf |archive-date=2013-04-04}}</ref> However, numerous ] and systematic reviews have shown significant evidence for the effects of stereotype threat, though the phenomenon defies over-simplistic characterization.<ref name="Pennington-2016">{{cite journal |vauthors=Pennington CR, Heim D, Levy AR, Larkin DT |date=2016-01-11 |title=Twenty Years of Stereotype Threat Research: A Review of Psychological Mediators |journal=PLOS ONE |volume=11 |issue=1 |pages=e0146487 |bibcode=2016PLoSO..1146487P |doi=10.1371/journal.pone.0146487 |pmc=4713435 |pmid=26752551 |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name="Nguyen-2008">{{cite journal |vauthors=Nguyen HH, Ryan AM |date=November 2008 |title=Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence |journal=The Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=93 |issue=6 |pages=1314–34 |doi=10.1037/a0012702 |pmid=19025250|s2cid=36769821 }}</ref><ref name="Walton-2009">{{Cite journal |last1=Walton |first1=Gregory M. |last2=Spencer |first2=Steven J. |date=2009-09-01 |title=Latent Ability: Grades and Test Scores Systematically Underestimate the Intellectual Ability of Negatively Stereotyped Students |journal=Psychological Science |volume=20 |issue=9 |pages=1132–1139 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02417.x |issn=0956-7976 |pmid=19656335 |s2cid=25810191|doi-access=free }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Gentile |first1=Ambra |last2=Boca |first2=Stefano |last3=Giammusso |first3=Isabella |date=2018-11-01 |title='You play like a Woman!' Effects of gender stereotype threat on Women's performance in physical and sport activities: A meta-analysis |journal=Psychology of Sport and Exercise |volume=39 |pages=95–103 |doi=10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.07.013 |s2cid=149490634 |issn=1469-0292}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Lamont |first1=Ruth A. |last2=Swift |first2=Hannah J. |last3=Abrams |first3=Dominic |year=2015 |title=A Review and Meta-Analysis of Age-Based Stereotype Threat: Negative Stereotypes, Not Facts, Do the Damage. |journal=Psychology and Aging |volume=30 |issue=1 |pages=180–193 |doi=10.1037/a0038586 |issn=1939-1498 |pmc=4360754 |pmid=25621742}}</ref><ref name="Picho-2013">{{Cite journal |last1=Picho |first1=Katherine |last2=Rodriguez |first2=Ariel |last3=Finnie |first3=Lauren |date=May 2013 |title=Exploring the Moderating Role of Context on the Mathematics Performance of Females Under Stereotype Threat: A Meta-Analysis |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/237000996 |journal=The Journal of Social Psychology |volume=153 |issue=3 |pages=299–333 |doi=10.1080/00224545.2012.737380 |pmid=23724702 |s2cid=45950675}}</ref><ref name="Liu-2020">{{Cite journal |last1=Liu |first1=Songqi |last2=Liu |first2=Pei |last3=Wang |first3=Mo |last4=Zhang |first4=Baoshan |date=July 2020 |title=Effectiveness of Stereotype Threat Interventions: A Meta-Analytic Review |url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/343149798 |journal=Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=currently in press |issue=6 |pages=921–949 |doi=10.1037/apl0000770 |pmid=32772526 |s2cid=221098319}}</ref>{{excessive citations inline|date=June 2024}} For instance, one meta-analysis found that with female subjects "subtle threat-activating cues produced the largest effect, followed by blatant and moderately explicit cues" while with minorities "moderately explicit stereotype threat-activating cues produced the largest effect, followed by blatant and subtle cues".<ref name="Nguyen-2008" />
Research published in 1997 indicates that part of the black-white gap in cognitive ability test scores is due to racial differences in test motivation.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Chan |first=D. |last2=Schmitt |first2=N. |last3=DeShon |first3=R. P. |last4=Clause |first4=C. S. |last5=Delbridge |first5=K. |date=April 1997 |title=Reactions to cognitive ability tests: the relationships between race, test performance, face validity perceptions, and test-taking motivation |journal=The Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=82 |issue=2 |pages=300–310 |issn=0021-9010 |pmid=9109288 |doi=10.1037/0021-9010.82.2.300}}</ref>


Some researchers have argued that studies of stereotype threat may in fact systematically under-represent its effects, since such studies measure "only that portion of psychological threat that research has identified and remedied. To the extent that unidentified or unremedied psychological threats further undermine performance, the results underestimate the bias."<ref name="Walton-2009"/>
However, attempts to replicate studies evincing significant effects of stereotype threat have not yielded the same results. In 2004 Sackett et al. found that eliminating stereotype threat does not eliminate the racial test performance gap, and in 2005 Tyson et al. found that African Americans have motivation similar to or even better than that of white Americans.<ref>{{Cite journal|title=It's Not "a Black Thing": Understanding the Burden of Acting White and Other Dilemmas of High Achievement|journal=American Sociological Review|volume=70|issue=4|pages=582–605|authors=Karolyn Tyson, William Darity Jr., and Domini R. Castellino|date=1 August 2005|doi=10.1177/000312240507000403}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal|title=On Interpreting Stereotype Threat as Accounting for African American-White Differences on Cognitive Tests|journal=American Psychologist|volume=59|issue=1|pages=7–13|authors=Paul R. Sackett, Chaitra M. Hardison, and Michael J. Cullen|date=January 2004|doi=10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.7|pmid=14736315}}</ref> Self-affirmation exercises promoted by research scientists such as Geoffrey L. Cohen have not been shown to be effective by attempts to replicate his studies.<ref>{{cite news|last1=Yong|first1=Ed|title=A Worrying Trend for Psychology's 'Simple Little Tricks'|url=https://www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2016/09/can-simple-tricks-mobilise-voters-and-help-students/499109|accessdate=11 September 2016|work=]|date=9 September 2016}}
*''See also'':
:* {{Cite journal | last1 = Borman | first1 = Geoffrey D. | last2 = Grigg | first2 = Jeffrey | last3 = Hanselman | first3 = Paul | title = An effort to close achievement gaps at scale through self-affirmation | journal = ] | volume = 38 | issue = 1 | pages = 21–42 | doi = 10.3102/0162373715581709 | date = March 2016 | ref = harv }}
:* {{Cite journal | last1 = Hanselman | first1 = Paul | last2 = Rozek | first2 = Christopher S. | last3 = Grigg | first3 = Jeffrey | last4 = Borman | first4 = Geoffrey D. | title = New evidence on self-affirmation effects and theorized sources of heterogeneity from large-scale replications | journal = ] | volume = 109 | issue = 3 | pages = 405–424 | doi = 10.1037/edu0000141 | pmid = 28450753 | pmc = 5403146 | date = April 2017 | ref = harv }} </ref> A 2015 meta-analysis conducted by Flore & Wicherts of studies on the relationship between gender and stereotype threat found the observed estimates to be inflated by ], arguing the true effect to be most likely near zero.<ref>{{Cite journal|title=Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis|authors=Paulette C. Flore and Jelte M. Wicherts|date=February 2015|journal=Journal of School Psychology|url=|doi=10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002|pmid=25636259|volume=53|issue=1|pages=25–44}}</ref>


==Research into the possible genetic influences on test score differences== ==Research into possible genetic factors==
{{see also|Heritability of IQ}} {{see also|Heritability of IQ}}
Although IQ differences between individuals have been shown to have a large hereditary component, it does not follow that mean group-level disparities (between-group differences) in IQ necessarily have a genetic basis.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |last3=Blair |first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens |first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |date=2012 |title=Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments. |journal=American Psychologist |language=en |volume=67 |issue=2 |pages=130–159 |doi=10.1037/a0026699 |issn=1935-990X |pmid=22233090}}</ref><ref name="Nisbett-2012" /> The scientific consensus is that there is no evidence for a genetic component behind IQ differences between racial groups.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Bird |first1=Kevin |last2=Jackson |first2=John P. |last3=Winston |first3=Andrew S. |date=2024 |title=Confronting Scientific Racism in Psychology: Lessons from Evolutionary Biology and Genetics |url=https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Famp0001228 |journal=American Psychologist |volume=79 |issue=4 |pages=497–508 |doi=10.1037/amp0001228 |pmid=39037836 |quote=Recent articles claim that the folk categories of race are genetically meaningful divisions, and that evolved genetic differences among races and nations are important for explaining immutable differences in cognitive ability, educational attainment, crime, sexual behavior, and wealth; all claims that are opposed by a strong scientific consensus to the contrary.}}</ref>{{sfn|Ceci|Williams|2009|pages=788–789, "There is an emerging consensus about racial and gender equality in genetic determinants of intelligence; most researchers, including ourselves, agree that genes do not explain between-group differences"}}{{sfn|Hunt|2010|page=447|ps= , "It is worth remembering that no genes related to difference in cognitive skills across the various racial and ethnic groups have ever been discovered. The argument for genetic differences has been carried forward largely by circumstantial evidence. Of course, tomorrow afternoon genetic mechanisms producing racial and ethnic differences in intelligence might be discovered, but there have been a lot of investigations, and tomorrow has not come for quite some time now."}}{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011||pages=334–338, 344}}<ref name="Nisbett-2012">{{cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |last3=Blair |first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens |first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |author-link5=Jim Flynn (academic) |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |author-link6=Diane F. Halpern |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |date=2012 |title=Group differences in IQ are best understood as environmental in origin |journal=American Psychologist |volume=67 |number=6 |pages=503–504 |doi=10.1037/a0029772 |issn=0003-066X |pmid=22963427 |author-link1=Richard E. Nisbett}}</ref><ref name="Kaplan-2015">{{Cite journal |last=Kaplan |first=Jonathan Michael |date=January 2015 |title=Race, IQ, and the search for statistical signals associated with so-called "X"-factors: environments, racism, and the "hereditarian hypothesis" |journal=Biology & Philosophy |language=en |volume=30 |issue=1 |pages=1–17 |doi=10.1007/s10539-014-9428-0 |s2cid=85351431 |issn=0169-3867}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Panofsky |first1=Aaron |last2=Dasgupta |first2=Kushan |last3=Iturriaga |first3=Nicole |title=How White nationalists mobilize genetics: From genetic ancestry and human biodiversity to counterscience and metapolitics |journal=American Journal of Physical Anthropology |year=2021 |volume=175 |issue=2 |pages=387–398 |doi=10.1002/ajpa.24150 |issn=0002-9483 |pmid=32986847 |pmc=9909835 |quote=he claims that genetics defines racial groups and makes them different, that IQ and cultural differences among racial groups are caused by genes, and that racial inequalities within and between nations are the inevitable outcome of long evolutionary processes are neither new nor supported by science (either old or new). |doi-access=free}}</ref><ref name="LewontinSameTitle">{{cite journal |last1=Lewontin |first1=Richard C. |title=Race and Intelligence |journal=Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists |date=March 1970 |volume=26 |issue=3 |pages=2–8 |doi=10.1080/00963402.1970.11457774 |bibcode=1970BuAtS..26c...2L |url=https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00963402.1970.11457774 |access-date=26 April 2021 |archive-date=10 June 2021 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210610120351/https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/00963402.1970.11457774 |url-status=live }}</ref>{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} Growing evidence indicates that environmental factors, not genetic ones, explain the racial IQ gap.{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}}<ref name="Nisbett-2012" />{{sfn|Nevid|2014|page=271}}<ref name="Kaplan-2015" />
Ongoing research aims to understand the contribution of genes to differences in intelligence. Currently there is no non-circumstantial evidence that the test score gap has a genetic component,{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|p=358}}{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012}}{{sfn|Kaplan|2015}} although some researchers believe that the existing circumstantial evidence makes it plausible to believe that hard evidence for a genetic component will eventually appear.{{sfn|Hunt|2010|pages=436, 447}} Growing evidence indicates that environmental factors, not genetic ones, are more important in explaining the racial IQ gap.{{sfn|Nevid|2014|page=271}} Several lines of investigation have been followed in the attempt to ascertain whether there is a genetic component to the test score gap as well as its relative contribution to the magnitude of the gap.


===Genetics of race and intelligence=== ===Genetics of race and intelligence===
{{main|Race and genetics}} {{main|Race and genetics}}
Geneticist ] argues that the question about the possible genetic effects on the test score gap is muddled by the general focus on "race" rather than on populations defined by gene frequency or by geographical proximity, and by the general insistence on phrasing the question in terms of heritability.<ref name="Templeton 2001">{{harvnb|Templeton|2001}}</ref> Templeton points out that racial groups neither represent ] nor distinct ], and that therefore there is no basis for making claims about the general intelligence of races.<ref name="Templeton 2001"/> From this point of view the search for possible genetic influences on the black-white test score gap is a priori flawed, because there is no genetic material shared by all Africans or by all Europeans. {{harvp|Mackintosh|2011}}, however, argues that by using genetic cluster analysis to correlate gene frequencies with continental populations it might be possible to show that African populations have a higher frequency of certain genetic variants that contribute to an average lower intelligence. Such a hypothetical situation could hold without all Africans carrying the same genes or belonging to a single evolutionary lineage. According to Mackintosh, a biological basis for the gap thus cannot be ruled out on a priori grounds. Geneticist ] argued that the question about the possible genetic effects on the test score gap is muddled by the general focus on "race" rather than on populations defined by gene frequency or by geographical proximity, and by the general insistence on phrasing the question in terms of heritability.<ref name="Templeton 2001">{{harvnb|Templeton|2001}}</ref> Templeton pointed out that racial groups neither represent ] nor distinct ], and that therefore there is no basis for making claims about the general intelligence of races.<ref name="Templeton 2001"/> He argued that, for these reasons, the search for possible genetic influences on the black–white test score gap is ''a priori'' flawed, because there is no genetic material shared by all Africans or by all Europeans. {{harvp|Mackintosh|2011}}, on the other hand, argued that by using genetic cluster analysis to correlate gene frequencies with continental populations it might be possible to show that African populations have a higher frequency of certain genetic variants that contribute to differences in average intelligence. Such a hypothetical situation could hold without all Africans carrying the same genes or belonging to a single evolutionary lineage. According to Mackintosh, a biological basis for the observed gap in IQ test performance thus cannot be ruled out on ''a priori'' grounds.{{Page needed|date=January 2022}}


{{harvtxt|Hunt|2010|page=447}} noted that "no genes related to difference in cognitive skills have across the various racial and ethnic groups have ever been discovered. The argument for genetic differences has been carried forward largely by circumstantial evidence. Of course, tomorrow afternoon genetic mechanisms producing racial and ethnic differences in intelligence might be discovered, but there have been a lot of investigations, and tomorrow has not come for quite some time now." {{harvtxt|Mackintosh|2011|page=344}} concurred, noting that while several environmental factors have been shown to influence the IQ gap, the evidence for a genetic influence has been negligible. A 2012 review by {{harvp|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair| Dickens|2012a}} concluded that the entire IQ gap can be explained by known environmental factors, and Mackintosh found this view to be plausible.
Intelligence is a ]. This means that intelligence is under the influence of several genes, possibly several thousand. The effect of most individual genetic variants on intelligence is thought to be very small, well below 1% of the variance in ''g''. Current studies using ] have yielded little success in the search for genes influencing intelligence. ] is confident that QTLs responsible for the variation in IQ scores exist, but due to their small effect sizes, more powerful tools of analysis will be required to detect them.<ref>{{harvnb|Plomin|Kennedy|Craig|2005|p=513}}</ref> Others assert that no useful answers can be reasonably expected from such research before an understanding of the relation between DNA and human phenotypes emerges.<ref name="Cooper 2005">{{harvnb|Cooper|2005}}</ref> Several candidate genes have been proposed to have a relationship with intelligence.<ref>{{harvnb|Zinkstok et al.|2007}}</ref><ref>{{harvnb|Dick et al.|2007}}</ref> However, a review of candidate genes for intelligence published in {{harvp|Deary|Johnson|Houlihan|2009}} failed to find evidence of an association between these genes and general intelligence, stating "there is still almost no replicated evidence concerning the individual genes, which have variants that contribute to intelligence differences".<ref name="Deary, Johnson & Houlihan 2009">{{harvnb|Deary|Johnson|Houlihan|2009}}</ref> In 2001, a review in the '']'' refuted eight major premises on which the hereditarian view regarding race and intelligence is based.<ref name="Onwegbuzie & Daley 2001">{{harvnb|Onwuegbuzie|Daley|2001}}</ref>


More recent research attempting to identify genetic loci associated with individual-level differences in IQ has yielded promising results, which led the editorial board of '']'' to issue a statement differentiating this research from the "racist" pseudoscience which it acknowledged has dogged intelligence research since its inception.<ref name="Nature-2017" /> It characterized the idea of genetically determined differences in intelligence between races as definitively false.<ref name="Nature-2017">{{Cite journal |date=25 May 2017 |title=Intelligence research should not be held back by its past |journal=Nature |volume=545 |issue=7655 |pages=385–386 |doi=10.1038/nature.2017.22021 |pmid=28541341 |bibcode=2017Natur.545R.385. |s2cid=4449918|doi-access=free }}</ref> Analysis of polygenic scores sampled from the 1000 Genomes Project has likewise found no evidence that intelligence was under diversifying selection in Africans and Europeans, suggesting that genetic differences make up a negligible component of the observed Black-White gap in IQ.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Bird |first=Kevin A. |date=2 February 2021 |title=No support for the hereditarian hypothesis of the Black–White achievement gap using polygenic scores and tests for divergent selection |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/ajpa.24216 |journal=] |language=en |volume=175 |issue=2 |pages=465–476 |doi=10.1002/ajpa.24216 |pmid=33529393 |issn=0002-9483 |access-date=1 November 2024 |via=Wiley Online Library}}</ref>
A 2005 literature review article by Sternberg, Grigorenko and Kidd stated that no gene has been shown to be linked to intelligence, "so attempts to provide a compelling genetic link of race to intelligence are not feasible at this time".{{sfn|Sternberg|Grigorenko|Kidd|2005|p=46}} {{harvtxt|Hunt|2010|page=447}} and {{harvtxt|Mackintosh|2011|page=344}} concurred, both scholars noting that while several environmental factors have been shown to influence the IQ gap, the evidence for a genetic influence has been circumstantial, and according to Mackintosh negligible. Mackintosh however suggests that it may never become possible to account satisfyingly for the relative contributions of genetic and environmental factors. The 2012 review by {{harvp|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair| Dickens|2012}} concluded that "Almost no genetic polymorphisms have been discovered that are consistently associated with variation in IQ in the normal range". Hunt and several other researchers however maintain that genetic causes cannot be ruled out, and that new evidence may yet show a genetic contribution to the gap. Hunt concurs with Rushton and Jensen who considered the 100% environmental hypothesis to be impossible. Nonetheless, Nisbett and colleagues (2012) consider the entire IQ gap to be explained by the environmental factors that have thus far been demonstrated to influence it, and Mackintosh does not find this view to be unreasonable.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012}}


===Heritability within and between groups=== ===Heritability within and between groups===
].]] ].|330x330px]]


] of intelligence have reported high heritability values. However, these studies are based on questionable assumptions.{{sfn|Carson|Beckwith|2001}} When used in the context of human ], the term "heritability" is highly misleading, as it does not convey any information about the relative importance of genetic or environmental factors on the development of a given trait, nor does it convey the extent to which that trait is genetically determined.{{sfn|Moore|Shenk|2016}} Arguments in support of a genetic explanation of racial differences in IQ are sometimes fallacious. For instance, hereditarians have sometimes cited the failure of known environmental factors to account for such differences, or the high heritability of intelligence within races, as evidence that racial differences in IQ are genetic.<ref>{{harvnb|Mackenzie|1984}}</ref> ] of intelligence have reported high heritability values. However, these studies have been criticized for being based on questionable assumptions.<ref>{{Cite book |last1=Carson |first1=Michael |title='Race', IQ and Genes |last2=Beckwith |first2=Jon |date=2001 |publisher=John Wiley & Sons, Ltd |isbn=978-0-470-01590-2 |pages=1–5 |language=en |doi=10.1002/9780470015902.a0005689.pub3}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Beckwith |first1=Jon |last2=Morris |first2=Corey A. |date=December 2008 |title=Twin Studies of Political Behavior: Untenable Assumptions? |journal=Perspectives on Politics |language=en |volume=6 |issue=4 |pages=785–791 |doi=10.1017/S1537592708081917 |s2cid=55630117 |issn=1541-0986 }}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Kamin |first1=Leon J. |last2=Goldberger |first2=Arthur S. |date=February 2002 |title=Twin Studies in Behavioral Research: A Skeptical View |journal=Theoretical Population Biology |volume=61 |issue=1 |pages=83–95 |doi=10.1006/tpbi.2001.1555 |pmid=11895384 |bibcode=2002TPBio..61...83K |issn=0040-5809}}</ref> When used in the context of human ], the term "heritability" can be misleading, as it does not necessarily convey information about the relative importance of genetic or environmental factors on the development of a given trait, nor does it convey the extent to which that trait is genetically determined.{{sfn|Moore|Shenk|2016}} Arguments in support of a genetic explanation of racial differences in IQ are sometimes fallacious. For instance, hereditarians have sometimes cited the failure of known environmental factors to account for such differences, or the high heritability of intelligence within races, as evidence that racial differences in IQ are genetic.<ref>{{harvnb|Mackenzie|1984}}</ref>


Psychometricians have found that intelligence is substantially heritable within populations, with 30–50% of variance in IQ scores in early childhood being attributable to genetic factors in analyzed US populations, increasing to 75–80% by late adolescence.<ref name="Neisser et al. 1996"/><ref name="Deary, Johnson & Houlihan 2009"/> In biology heritability is defined as the ratio of variation attributable to genetic differences in an observable ] to the trait's total observable variation. The heritability of a trait describes the proportion of variation in the trait that is attributable to genetic factors within a particular population. A heritability of 1 indicates that variation correlates fully with genetic variation and a heritability of 0 indicates that there is no correlation between the trait and genes at all. In psychological testing, heritability tends to be understood as the degree of correlation between the results of a test taker and those of their biological parents. However, since high heritability is simply a correlation between traits and genes, it does not describe the causes of heritability which in humans can be either genetic or environmental. Psychometricians have found that intelligence is substantially heritable within populations, with 30–50% of variance in IQ scores in early childhood being attributable to genetic factors in analyzed US populations, increasing to 75–80% by late adolescence.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}}<ref name="Deary, Johnson & Houlihan 2009">{{harvnb|Deary|Johnson|Houlihan|2009}}</ref> In biology heritability is defined as the ratio of variation attributable to genetic differences in an observable ] to the trait's total observable variation. The heritability of a trait describes the proportion of variation in the trait that is attributable to genetic factors within a particular population. A heritability of 1 indicates that variation correlates fully with genetic variation and a heritability of 0 indicates that there is no correlation between the trait and genes at all. In psychological testing, heritability tends to be understood as the degree of correlation between the results of a test taker and those of their biological parents. However, since high heritability is simply a correlation between child and parents, it does not describe the causes of heritability which in humans can be either genetic or environmental.


Therefore, a high heritability measure does not imply that a trait is genetic or unchangeable, however, as environmental factors that affect all group members equally will not be measured by heritability and the heritability of a trait may also change over time in response to changes in the distribution of genes and environmental factors.<ref name="Neisser et al. 1996"/> High heritability also doesn't imply that all of the heritability is genetically determined, but can also be due to environmental differences that affect only a certain genetically defined group (indirect heritability).<ref name="Block 2002">{{harvnb|Block|2002}}</ref> The figure to the left demonstrates how heritability works. In both gardens the difference between tall and short cornstalks is 100% heritable as cornstalks that are genetically disposed for growing tall will become taller than those without this disposition, but the difference in height between the cornstalks to the left and those on the right is 100% environmental as it is due to different nutrients being supplied to the two gardens. Hence the causes of differences within a group and between groups may not be the same, even when looking at traits that are highly heritable.<ref name="Block 2002" /> In his ], ] further illustrated this with the example of women wearing earrings:{{quote|To borrow an example from Ned Block, "some years ago when only women wore earrings, the heritability of having an earring was high because differences in whether a person had an earring was due to a chromosomal difference, XX vs. XY." No one has yet suggested that wearing earrings, or ties, is "in our genes," an inescapable fate that environment cannot influence, "dooming the liberal notion."<ref name=Rollback> Noam Chomsky, 1995</ref>}} Therefore, a high heritability measure does not imply that a trait is genetic or unchangeable. In addition, environmental factors that affect all group members equally will not be measured by heritability, and the heritability of a trait may also change over time in response to changes in the distribution of genetic and environmental factors.{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} High heritability does not imply that all of the heritability is genetically determined; rather, it can also be due to environmental differences that affect only a certain genetically defined group (indirect heritability).<ref name="Block 2002">{{harvnb|Block|2002}}</ref>


The figure to the right demonstrates how heritability works. In each of the two gardens the difference between tall and short cornstalks is 100% heritable, as cornstalks that are genetically disposed for growing tall will become taller than those without this disposition. But the difference in height between the cornstalks to the left and those on the right is 100% environmental, as it is due to different nutrients being supplied to the two gardens. Hence, the causes of differences within a group and between groups may not be the same, even when looking at traits that are highly heritable.<ref name="Block 2002" />
In regards to the IQ gap the question becomes whether racial groups can be shown to be influenced by different environmental factors that may account for the observed differences between them. Jensen originally argued that given the high heritability of IQ the only way that the IQ gap could be explained as caused by the environment would be if it could be shown that all blacks were subject to a single "x-factor" which affected no white populations while affecting all black populations equally.<ref>{{cite book|last=Jensen|first=A.R.|year=1998|title=''The ''g'' factor: The science of mental ability''|location=Westport, CT|publisher=Praeger|isbn=978-0-275-96103-9|ref=harv|pages=|url=https://archive.org/details/gfactorscienc00jens/page/456}}</ref> Jensen considered the existence of such an x-factor to be extremely improbable, but Flynn's discovery of the Flynn effect showed that in spite of high heritability environmental factors could cause considerable disparities in IQ between generations of the same population, showing that the existence of such an x-factor was not only possible but real.{{sfn|Flynn|2012|pages=134–141}}


=== Spearman's hypothesis ===
Jensen has also argued that heritability of traits rises with age as the genetic potential of individuals becomes expressed. He sees this as related to the fact that the IQ gap between white and black test takers has been shown to appear gradually, with the gap widening as cohorts reach adulthood. This he sees as a further argument in favor of Spearman's hypothesis (see section below).

In contrast, Dickens and Flynn argued that the conventional interpretation ignores the role of ] between factors, such as those with a small initial IQ advantage, genetic or environmental, seeking out more stimulating environments which will gradually greatly increase their advantage, which, as one consequence in their alternative model, would mean that the "heritability" figure is only in part due to direct effects of genotype on IQ.{{sfn|Hunt|Carlson|2007}}{{sfn|Nisbett|2009|p=212}}{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2001}}

Today researchers such as {{harvp|Hunt|2010}}, {{harvp|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012}} and {{harvp|Mackintosh|2011}} consider that rather than a single factor accounting for the entire gap, probably many different environmental factors differ systematically between the environments of white and black people converge to create part of the gap and perhaps all of it. They argue that it does not make sense to talk about a single universal heritability figure for IQ, rather, they state, heritability of IQ varies between and within groups. They point specifically to studies showing a higher heritability of test scores in white and medium-high SES families, but considerably lower heritability for black and low-SES families. This they interpret to mean that children who grow up with limited resources do not get to develop their full genetic potential.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012}}

Multiple studies have been conducted over the past several decades to survey scientific estimates on the heritability of the IQ gap. A ] in 1988 found that 45% of the scientists they questioned believed the gap to be "a product of genetic and environmental variation," 15% and 1% respectively "entirely to environmental" and "genetic variation," while the remaining 38% either declined to answer or stated that the evidence was inconclusive.<ref>Snyderman & Rothman (1987:255), cited in Eysenck (1994:66).</ref> The heritability of intelligence was estimated on average to be 59.6% for white Americans and 57.0% for black Americans among those who answered that the evidence was sufficiently conclusive.<ref>{{harvnb|Silverman|1991|p=250}}</ref> The ''Wall Street Journal'' published an ], signed by 52 professors specializing in intelligence and allied fields, that estimated the heritability of individual variation to range between 40–80%, but also stating that "there is no definitive answer" to explain the racial gap.<ref>{{Cite web|title=Mainstream Science on Intelligence: An Editorial With 52 Signatories, History, and Bibliography|author=Linda S. Gottfredson|publisher=University of Delaware|accessdate=12 January 2018|url=http://www1.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/1997mainstream.pdf}}</ref> Social psychologist ] criticized the report, arguing that it overstated the plausibility of genetic explanations and underestimated the extent of environmental differences between races.<ref>{{Citation|last=Campbell|first=Donald T.|authorlink=Donald T. Campbell|journal= Psychological Assessment|pages= 363–368|year= 1996|volume= 8|issue=4|title=Unresolved Issues in Measurement Validity : An Autobiographical Overview|doi=10.1037/1040-3590.8.4.363 }}</ref> A ] stated that there is more plausible evidence for an environmental than for a genetic explanation, but that there was "no adequate explanation" for the black-white IQ gap.<ref>{{Cite web|title=Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns|url=http://differentialclub.wdfiles.com/local--files/definitions-structure-and-measurement/Intelligence-Knowns-and-unknowns.pdf}}</ref><ref>{{Cite journal| doi = 10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77| year = 1996| last1 = Neisser | first1 = U.| last2 = Boodoo | first2 = G.| last3 = Bouchard | first3 = T. J., J.| last4 = Boykin | first4 = A. W.| last5 = Brody | first5 = N.| last6 = Ceci | first6 = S. J.| last7 = Halpern | first7 = D. F.| last8 = Loehlin | first8 = J. C.| last9 = Perloff | first9 = R.| last10 = Sternberg | first10 = R. J.| last11 = Urbina | first11 = S.| title = Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns| journal = American Psychologist| volume = 51| issue = 2| pages = 77–101 }}</ref> In a 2013 followup on Snyderman & Rothman, Rindermann et al. found the average and median estimates of the black-white IQ gap to be heritable by 47% and 50% respectively among surveyed scientists who believed that the available evidence allowed for a reasonable estimate. This survey however yielded a response rate of 18% (228 participants) compared to Snyderman & Rothman's 65% (661 participants).<ref>{{Cite web|title=2013 survey of expert opinion on intelligence|authors=Heiner Rindermann, Thomas R. Coyle, and David Becker|publisher=International Society for Intelligence Research, 2013|accessdate=12 January 2018|url=https://www.researchgate.net/publication/265167014}}</ref>

===Spearman's hypothesis===
{{Main|Spearman's hypothesis}} {{Main|Spearman's hypothesis}}
Spearman's hypothesis states that the magnitude of the black-white difference in tests of cognitive ability is entirely or mainly a function of the extent to which a test measures general mental ability, or ''g''. The hypothesis was first formalized by ] who devised the statistical Method of Correlated Vectors to test it. Jensen holds that if Spearman's hypothesis holds true then some cognitive tasks have a higher g-load than others, and that these tasks are exactly the tasks in which the gap between black and white test takers are greatest. Jensen, and other psychometricians such as Rushton and Lynn, take this to show that the cause of ''g'' and the cause of the gap are the same—in their view genetic differences.{{sfn|Rushton|Jensen|2005}} Spearman's hypothesis states that the magnitude of the black–white difference in tests of cognitive ability depends entirely or mainly on the extent to which a test measures general mental ability, or ''g''. The hypothesis was first formalized by ], who devised the statistical "method of correlated vectors" to test it. If Spearman's hypothesis holds true, then the cognitive tasks that have the highest ''g''-load are the tasks in which the gap between black and white test takers are greatest. Jensen and Rushton took this to show that the cause of ''g'' and the cause of the gap are the same—in their view, genetic differences.{{sfn|Rushton|Jensen|2005}}


{{harvtxt|Mackintosh|2011|pages=338–39}} acknowledges that Jensen and Rushton have shown a modest correlation between g-loading, heritability, and the test score gap, but he does not accept that this demonstrates a genetic origin of the gap. He points out that it is exactly in those the tests that Rushton and Jensen consider to have the highest g-loading and heritability such as the Wechsler that has seen the highest increases due to the Flynn effect. This suggests that they are also the most sensitive to environmental changes. And in turn, if the highly g-loaded tests are both more liable to environmental influences and as Jensen argues, the ones where the black-white gap is most pronounced, it suggests in fact contrary to Jensen's argument that the gap is most likely caused by environmental factors. Mackintosh also argues that Spearman's hypothesis, which he considers to be likely to be correct, simply shows that the test score gap is based on whatever cognitive faculty is central to intelligence, but not what this factor is. {{harvtxt|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012|page=146}} make the same point, noting also that the increase in the IQ scores of black test takers is necessarily also an increase in ''g''. {{harvtxt|Mackintosh|2011|pages=338–39}} acknowledges that Jensen and Rushton showed a modest correlation between ''g''-loading, heritability, and the test score gap, but does not agree that this demonstrates a genetic origin of the gap. Mackintosh argues that it is exactly those tests that Rushton and Jensen consider to have the highest ''g''-loading and heritability, such as the Wechsler test, that have seen the greatest increases in black performance due to the Flynn effect. This likely suggests that they are also the most sensitive to environmental changes, which undermines Jensen's argument that the black–white gap is most likely caused by genetic factors. {{harvtxt|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a|page=146}} make the same point, noting also that the increase in the IQ scores of black test takers necessarily indicates an increase in ''g''.

James {{harvtxt|Flynn|2012|pages=140–1}} argues that there is an inherent flaw in Jensen's argument that the correlation between g-loadings, test scores and heritability support a genetic cause of the gap. He points out that as the difficulty of a task increases a low performing group will naturally fall further behind, and heritability will therefore also naturally increase. The same holds for increases in performance which will first affect the least difficult tasks, but only gradually affect the most difficult ones. Flynn thus sees the correlation between in g-loading and the test score gap to offer no clue to the cause of the gap.{{sfn|Flynn|2010}}

{{harvtxt|Hunt|2010|page=415}} states that many of conclusions of Jensen, and his colleagues rest on the validity of Spearman's hypothesis, and the method of correlated vectors used to test it. Hunt points out that other researchers have found this method of calculation to produce false positive results, and that other statistical methods should be used instead. According to Hunt, Jensen and Rushton's frequent claim that Spearman's hypothesis should be regarded as empirical fact does not hold, and that new studies based on better statistical methods would be required to confirm or reject the hypothesis that the correlation between g-loading, heritability and the IQ gap is due to IQ gaps consisting mostly of g.


James Flynn argued that his findings undermine Spearman's hypothesis.<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Flynn |first1=J.R. |year=1999 |title=Searching for justice: the discovery of IQ gains over time |url=http://www.stat.columbia.edu/~gelman/stuff_for_blog/flynn.pdf |url-status=live |journal=American Psychologist |volume=54 |pages=5–9 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.54.1.5 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20100625085640/http://www.stat.columbia.edu/~gelman/stuff_for_blog/flynn.pdf |archive-date=25 June 2010 |access-date=26 October 2017}}</ref> In a 2006 study, he and William Dickens found that between 1972 and 2002 "The standard measure of the ''g'' gap between Blacks and Whites declined virtually in tandem with the IQ gap."{{sfn|Dickens|Flynn|2006}} Flynn also criticized Jensen's basic assumption that a correlation between ''g''-loading and test score gap implies a genetic cause for the gap.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Flynn |first=James R. |year=2010 |title=The spectacles through which I see the race and IQ debate |url=http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/flynn2010a.pdf |journal=Intelligence |volume=38 |issue=4 |pages=363–366 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2010.05.001 |access-date=2011-02-18 |archive-date=2020-12-07 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20201207224050/http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/flynn2010a.pdf |url-status=live }}</ref> In a 2014 suite of meta-analyses, along with co-authors Jan te Nijenhuis and Daniel Metzen, he showed that the same negative correlation between IQ gains and ''g''-loading obtains for cognitive deficits of known environmental cause: ], ], ], and ].<ref>{{Cite journal |last1=Flynn |first1=James R. |last2=te Nijenhuis |first2=Jan |last3=Metzen |first3=Daniel |date=2014 |title=The g beyond Spearman's g: Flynn's paradoxes resolved using four exploratory meta-analyses |url=https://james-flynn.net/wp-content/uploads/2020/12/flynn2014-The-g-beyond-Spearmans-g-Flynns-paradoxes-resolved-using-four-exploratory-meta-analyses.pdf |journal=Intelligence |volume=44 |pages=1–10|doi=10.1016/j.intell.2014.01.009 }}</ref>
===Adoption studies=== ===Adoption studies===
A number of studies have been done on the effect of similar rearing conditions on children from different races. The hypothesis is that by investigating whether black children adopted into white families demonstrated gains in IQ test scores relative to black children reared in black families. Depending on whether their test scores are more similar to their biological or adoptive families, that could be interpreted as either supporting a genetic or an environmental hypothesis. The main point of critique in studies like these however is whether the environment of black children—even when raised in white families—is truly comparable to the environment of white children. Several reviews of the ] literature has pointed out that it is perhaps impossible to avoid confounding of biological and environmental factors in this type of studies.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|p=337}} Given the differing heritability estimates in medium-high SES and low-SES families, {{harvtxt|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012|pages=134}} argue that adoption studies on the whole tend to overstate the role of genetics because they represent a restricted set of environments, mostly in the medium-high SES range. A number of IQ studies have been done on the effect of similar rearing conditions on children from different races. The hypothesis is that this can be determined by investigating whether black children adopted into white families demonstrated gains in IQ test scores relative to black children reared in black families. Depending on whether their test scores are more similar to their biological or adoptive families, that could be interpreted as supporting either a genetic or an environmental hypothesis. Critiques of such studies question whether the environment of black children—even when raised in white families—is truly comparable to the environment of white children. Several reviews of the ] literature have suggested that it is probably impossible to avoid confounding biological and environmental factors in this type of study.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|p=337}} Another criticism by {{harvtxt|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a|pages=134}} is that adoption studies on the whole tend to be carried out in a restricted set of environments, mostly in the medium-high SES range, where heritability is higher than in the low-SES range.


The ] (1976) examined the ] test scores of 122 ] children and 143 nonadopted children reared by advantaged white families. The children were restudied ten years later.<ref name="Weinberg 1992">{{harvnb|Weinberg|Scarr|Waldman|1992}}</ref>{{sfn|Scarr|Weinberg|1976}}{{sfn|Loehlin|2000|p=185}} The study found higher IQ for whites compared to blacks, both at age 7 and age 17.<ref name="Weinberg 1992"/> {{harvp|Rushton|Jensen|2005}} cite the Minnesota study as providing support to a genetic explanation. Nonetheless, acknowledging the existence of confounding factors, Scarr and Weinberg the authors of the original study, did not themselves consider that it provided support for either the hereditarian or environmentalist view.{{sfn|Scarr|Weinberg|1990}} The ] (1976) examined the ] test scores of 122 ] children and 143 nonadopted children reared by advantaged white families. The children were restudied ten years later.<ref name="Weinberg 1992">{{harvnb|Weinberg|Scarr|Waldman|1992}}</ref>{{sfn|Scarr|Weinberg|1976}}{{sfn|Loehlin|2000|p=185}} The study found higher IQ for white people compared to black people, both at age 7 and age 17.<ref name="Weinberg 1992"/> Acknowledging the existence of confounding factors, Scarr and Weinberg, the authors of the original study, did not consider that it provided support for either the hereditarian or environmentalist view.{{sfn|Scarr|Weinberg|1990}}


Three other adoption studies found contrary evidence to the Minnesota study, lending support to a mostly environmental hypothesis: Three other studies lend support to environmental explanations of group IQ differences:
*{{harvp|Eyferth|1961}} studied the out-of-wedlock children of black and white soldiers stationed in Germany after World War 2 and then raised by white German mothers in what has become known as the ]. He found no significant differences in average IQ between groups. *{{harvp|Eyferth|1961}} studied the out-of-wedlock children of black and white soldiers stationed in Germany after World War II who were then raised by white German mothers in what has become known as the ]. He found no significant differences in average IQ between groups.
*{{harvp|Tizard et al.|1972}} studied black (West Indian), white, and mixed-race children raised in British long-stay residential nurseries. Two out of three tests found no significant differences. One test found higher scores for non-whites. *{{harvp|Tizard et al.|1972}} studied black (West Indian), white, and mixed-race children raised in British long-stay residential nurseries. Two out of three tests found no significant differences. One test found higher scores for non-white people.
*{{harvp|Moore|1986}} compared black and mixed-race children adopted by either black or white middle-class families in the United States. Moore observed that 23 black and interracial children raised by white parents had a significantly higher mean score than 23 age-matched children raised by black parents (117 vs 104), and argued that differences in early socialization explained these differences. *{{harvp|Moore|1986}} compared black and mixed-race children adopted by either black or white middle-class families in the United States. Moore observed that 23 black and interracial children raised by white parents had a significantly higher mean score than 23 age-matched children raised by black parents (117 vs 104), and argued that differences in early socialization explained these differences.


Frydman and Lynn (1989) showed a mean IQ of 119 for Korean infants adopted by Belgian families. After correcting for the ], the IQ of the adopted Korean children was still 10 points higher than that of the Belgian children.{{sfn|Loehlin|2000|p=187}}<ref name="Frydman and Lynn">{{cite journal |author=Frydman and Lynn |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |volume=10 |issue=12 |pages=1323–1325 |year=1989 |title=The intelligence of Korean children adopted in Belgium |doi=10.1016/0191-8869(89)90246-8}}</ref>
Rushton and Jensen have argued that unlike the Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study, these studies did not retest the children post-adolescence when heritability of IQ would presumably be higher.<ref name="Rushton & Jensen 2010"/><ref name="Rushton & Jensen 2005"/> {{harvtxt|Nisbett|2009|p=226}} however point out that the difference in heritability between ages 7 and 17 are quite small, and that consequently this is no reason to disregard Moore's findings.

Frydman and Lynn (1989) showed a mean IQ of 119 for Korean infants adopted by Belgian families. After correcting for the ], the IQ of the adopted Korean children was still 10 points higher than the indigenous Belgian children.{{sfn|Loehlin|2000|p=187}}{{sfn|Rushton|Jensen|2005}}<ref name="Frydman and Lynn">{{cite journal |author=Frydman and Lynn |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |volume=10 |issue=12 |pages=1323–1325|year=1989 |url= |title=The intelligence of Korean children adopted in Belgium|ref=harv |doi=10.1016/0191-8869(89)90246-8}}</ref>


Reviewing the evidence from adoption studies, Mackintosh finds that environmental and genetic variables remain confounded and considers evidence from adoption studies inconclusive, and fully compatible with a 100% environmental explanation.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|page=337}} Similarly, Drew Thomas argues that race differences in IQ that appear in adoption studies are in fact an artifact of methodology, and that East Asian IQ advantages and black IQ disadvantages disappear when this is controlled for.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Thomas |first1=Drew |year=2017 |title=Racial IQ Differences among Transracial Adoptees: Fact or Artifact? |journal=Journal of Intelligence |volume=5 |issue=1 |page=1 |doi=10.3390/jintelligence5010001 |pmid=31162392 |pmc=6526420 |doi-access=free}}</ref>
Reviewing the evidence from adoption studies Mackintosh considers the studies by Tizard and Eyferth to be inconclusive, and the Minnesota study to be consistent only with a partial genetic hypothesis. On the whole he finds that environmental and genetic variables remain confounded and considers evidence from adoption studies inconclusive on the whole, and fully compatible with a 100% environmental explanation.{{sfn|Mackintosh|2011|page=337}}


===Racial admixture studies=== ===Racial admixture studies===
Most people have an ancestry from different geographic regions, particularly African Americans typically have ancestors from both Africa and Europe, with, on average, 20% of their genome inherited from European ancestors.<ref>{{harvnb|Bryc et al.|2009}}</ref> If racial IQ gaps have a partially genetic basis, one might expect blacks with a higher degree of European ancestry to score higher on IQ tests than blacks with less European ancestry, because the genes inherited from European ancestors would likely include some genes with a positive effect on IQ.{{sfn|Loehlin|2000}} Geneticist ] has argued that an experiment based on the Mendelian "common garden" design where specimens with different hybrid compositions are subjected to the same environmental influences, would be the only way to definitively show a causal relation between genes and IQ. Summarizing the findings of admixture studies, he concludes that it has shown no significant correlation between any cognitive and the degree of African or European ancestry.{{sfn|Templeton|2001}} Most people have ancestry from different geographical regions. In particular, African Americans typically have ancestors from both Africa and Europe, with, on average, 20% of their genome inherited from European ancestors.<ref>{{harvnb|Bryc et al.|2009}}</ref> If racial IQ gaps have a partially genetic basis, one might expect black people with a higher degree of European ancestry to score higher on IQ tests than black people with less European ancestry, because the genes inherited from European ancestors would likely include some genes with a positive effect on IQ.{{sfn|Loehlin|2000}} Geneticist ] has argued that an experiment based on the Mendelian "common garden" design, where specimens with different hybrid compositions are subjected to the same environmental influences, are the only way to definitively show a causal relation between genes and group differences in IQ. Summarizing the findings of admixture studies, he concludes that they have shown no significant correlation between any cognitive ability and the degree of African or European ancestry.{{sfn|Templeton|2001}}


Studies have employed different ways of measuring or approximating relative degrees of ancestry from Africa and Europe. One set of studies have used skin color as a measure, and other studies have used blood groups. {{harvp|Loehlin|2000}} surveys the literature and argues that the blood groups studies may be seen as providing some support to the genetic hypothesis, even though the correlation between ancestry and IQ was quite low. He finds that studies by {{harvp|Eyferth|1961}}, Willerman, Naylor & Myrianthopoulos (1970) did not find a correlation between degree of African/European ancestry and IQ. The latter study did find a difference based on the race of the mother, with children of white mothers with black fathers scoring higher than children of black mothers and white fathers. Loehlin considers that such a finding is compatible with either a genetic or an environmental cause. All in all Loehlin finds admixture studies inconclusive and recommends more research. Studies have employed different ways of measuring or approximating relative degrees of ancestry from Africa and Europe. Some studies have used skin color as a measure, and others have used blood groups. {{harvp|Loehlin|2000}} surveys the literature and argues that the blood groups studies may be seen as providing some support to the genetic hypothesis, even though the correlation between ancestry and IQ was quite low. He finds that studies by {{harvp|Eyferth|1961}}, Willerman, Naylor & Myrianthopoulos (1970) did not find a correlation between degree of African/European ancestry and IQ. The latter study did find a difference based on the race of the mother, with children of white mothers with black fathers scoring higher than children of black mothers and white fathers. Loehlin considers that such a finding is compatible with either a genetic or an environmental cause. All in all Loehlin finds admixture studies inconclusive and recommends more research.


Reviewing the evidence from admixture studies {{harvp|Hunt|2010}} considers it to be inconclusive because of too many uncontrolled variables. {{harvtxt|Mackintosh|2011|p=338}} quotes a statement by {{harvp|Nisbett|2009}} to the effect that admixture studies have not provided a shred of evidence in favor of a genetic basis for the IQ gap.
Another study cited by {{harvp|Rushton|Jensen|2005}}, and by {{harvp|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012}}, was {{harvp|Moore|1986}} study which found that adopted mixed-race children's has test scores identical to children with two black parents—receiving no apparent "benefit" from their white ancestry. Rushton and Jensen consider admixture studies to have provided overall support for a genetic explanation though this view is not shared by {{harvp|Loehlin|2000}}, {{harvp|Nisbett|2009}}, {{harvp|Hunt|2010}}, {{harvp|Mackintosh|2011}}, nor by {{harvp|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012}}.

Reviewing the evidence from admixture studies {{harvp|Hunt|2010}} considers it to be inconclusive because of too many uncontrolled variables. {{harvtxt|Mackintosh|2011|p=338}} quotes a statement by {{harvp|Nisbett|2009}} to the effect that admixture studies have not provided a shred of evidence in favor of a genetic basis for the gap.


===Mental chronometry=== ===Mental chronometry===
{{Main|Mental chronometry}} {{Main|Mental chronometry}}
] measures the elapsed time between the presentation of a sensory stimulus and the subsequent behavioral response by the participant. These studies have shown inconsistent results when comparing black and white populations groups, with some studies showing whites outperforming blacks, and others showing blacks outperforming whites.{{sfn|Sheppard|Vernon|2008}}
] measures the elapsed time between the presentation of a sensory stimulus and the subsequent behavioral response by the participant. This reaction time (RT) is considered a measure of the speed and efficiency with which the brain processes information.<ref name="Jensen 2006">{{harvnb|Jensen|2006}}</ref> Scores on most types of RT tasks tend to correlate with scores on standard IQ tests as well as with ''g'', and no relationship has been found between RT and any other psychometric factors independent of ''g''.<ref name="Jensen 2006"/> The strength of the correlation with IQ varies from one RT test to another, but ] gives 0.40 as a typical correlation under favorable conditions.<ref>{{harvnb|Eysenck|1987}}</ref> According to Jensen individual differences in RT have a substantial genetic component, and heritability is higher for performance on tests that correlate more strongly with IQ.<ref name="Jensen 1998">{{harvnb|Jensen|1998}}</ref> Nisbett argues that some studies have found correlations closer to 0.2, and that the correlation is not always found.<ref name="Nisbett 2009">{{harvnb|Nisbett|2009}}</ref><!--page number needed-->


Several studies have found differences between races in average reaction times. These studies have generally found that reaction times among black, Asian and white children follow the same pattern as IQ scores.{{sfn|Lynn|Vanhanen|2002}}{{sfn|Jensen|Whang|1993}}{{sfn|Pesta|Poznanski|2008}} Black-white differences in reaction time, however, tend to be small (average ] .18).{{sfn|Sheppard|Vernon|2008}} {{harvp|Rushton|Jensen|2005}} have argued that reaction time is independent of culture and that the existence of race differences in average reaction time is evidence that the cause of racial IQ gaps is partially genetic instead of entirely cultural. Responding to this argument in ''Intelligence and How to Get It'', Nisbett has pointed to the {{harvp|Jensen|Whang|1993}} study in which a group of Chinese Americans had longer reaction times than a group of European Americans, despite having higher IQs. Nisbett also mentions findings in {{harvp|Flynn|1991}} and {{harvp|Deary|2001}} suggesting that movement time (the measure of how long it takes a person to move a finger after making the decision to do so) correlates with IQ just as strongly as reaction time, and that average movement time is faster for blacks than for whites.{{sfn|Nisbett|2009|pp=221–2}} {{harvtxt|Mackintosh|2011|page=339}} considers reaction time evidence unconvincing and points out that other cognitive tests that also correlate well with IQ show no disparity at all, for example the habituation/] test. And he points out that studies show that rhesus monkeys have shorter reaction times than American college students, suggesting that different reaction times may not tell us anything useful about intelligence. Arthur Jensen argued that this reaction time (RT) is a measure of the speed and efficiency with which the brain processes information,<ref name="Jensen 2006">{{harvnb|Jensen|2006}}</ref> and that scores on most types of RT tasks tend to correlate with scores on standard IQ tests as well as with ''g''.<ref name="Jensen 2006" /> Nisbett argues that some studies have found correlations closer to 0.2, and that a correlation is not always found.<ref name="Nisbett 2009">{{harvnb|Nisbett|2009}}</ref> Nisbett points to the {{harvp|Jensen|Whang|1993}} study in which a group of Chinese Americans had longer reaction times than a group of European Americans, despite having higher IQs. Nisbett also mentions findings in {{harvp|Flynn|1991}} and {{harvp|Deary|2001}} suggesting that movement time (the measure of how long it takes a person to move a finger after making the decision to do so) correlates with IQ just as strongly as reaction time, and that average movement time is faster for black people than for white people.{{sfn|Nisbett|2009|pp=221–2}} {{harvtxt|Mackintosh|2011|page=339}} considers reaction time evidence unconvincing and comments that other cognitive tests that also correlate well with IQ show no disparity at all, for example the habituation/] test. He further comments that studies show that rhesus monkeys have shorter reaction times than American college students, suggesting that different reaction times may not tell us anything useful about intelligence.


===Brain size=== ===Brain size===
{{main|Brain size}} {{main|Brain size}}
A number of studies have reported a moderate statistical correlation between differences in IQ and brain size between individuals in the same group.{{sfn|Deary|Penke|Johnson|2010}}{{sfn|McDaniel|2005}} And some scholars have reported differences in average brain sizes between Africans, Europeans, and Asians.{{sfn|Ho et al.|1980}} J. P. Rushton has argued that Africans on average have smaller brain cases and brains than Europeans, that Europeans have smaller brains than East Asians, and that this is evidence that the gap is biological in nature. Critics of Rushton have argued that Rushton's arguments rest on outdated data collected by unsound methods and should be considered invalid.{{sfn|Lieberman|2001}} Recent reviews by {{harvp|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}} and {{harvp|Mackintosh|2011}} consider that current data does show an average difference in brain size and head-circumference between American blacks and whites, but question whether this has any relevance for the IQ gap. Nisbett et al. argue that crude brain size is unlikely to be a good measure of IQ; for example, brain size also differs between men and women, but without well-documented differences in IQ. At the same time newborn black children have the same average brain size as whites, suggesting that the difference in average size could be accounted for by differences in postnatal environment. Several factors that reduce brain size have been demonstrated to disproportionately affect black children.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}} A number of studies have reported a moderate statistical correlation between differences in IQ and brain size between individuals in the same group.{{sfn|Deary|Penke|Johnson|2010}}{{sfn|McDaniel|2005}} Some scholars have reported differences in average brain sizes between racial groups,{{sfn|Ho et al.|1980}} although this is unlikely to be a good measure of IQ as brain size also differs between men and women, but without significant differences in IQ.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}} At the same time newborn black children have the same average brain size as white children, suggesting that the difference in average size could be accounted for by differences in environment.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}} Several environmental factors that reduce brain size have been demonstrated to disproportionately affect black children.{{sfn|Nisbett|Aronson|Blair|Dickens|2012a}}

] states that brain size is found to have a correlation of about .35 with intelligence among whites and cites studies showing that genes may account for as much as 90% of individual variation in brain size. According to Hunt, race differences in average brain size could potentially be an important argument for a possible genetic contribution to racial IQ gaps. Nonetheless, Hunt notes that Rushton's head size data would account for a difference of .09 standard deviations between black and white average test scores, less than a tenth of the 1.0 standard deviation gap in average scores that is observed.{{sfn|Hunt|Carlson|2007}}{{sfn|Hunt|2010|pp=433–434}} Wicherts, Borsboom, & Dolan (2010) argue that black-white differences in brain size are insufficient to explain 91% to 95% of the black-white IQ gap.{{sfn|Wicherts|Borsboom|Dolan|2010b}}


===Archaeological data=== ===Archaeological data===
Archaeological evidence does not support claims by Rushton and others that blacks' cognitive ability was inferior to whites' during prehistoric times as a result of evolution.{{sfn|MacEachern|2006}} Archaeological evidence does not support claims by Rushton and others that black people's cognitive ability was inferior to white people's during prehistoric times.{{sfn|MacEachern|2006}}


==Policy relevance and ethics== ==Policy relevance and ethics==
{{Main|Intelligence and public policy}} {{Main|Intelligence and public policy}}
The 1996 report of the APA commented on the ] of research on race and intelligence.<ref name="Hunt & Carlson 2007">{{harvnb|Hunt|Carlson|2007}}</ref> {{harvp|Gray|Thompson|2004}} as well as {{harvp|Hunt|Carlson|2007}} have also discussed different possible ethical guidelines.<ref name="Hunt & Carlson 2007"/><ref>{{harvnb|Gray|Thompson|2004}}</ref>{{primary source inline|date=February 2013}} ] in 2009 featured two editorials on the ethics of research in race and intelligence by ] (against) and ] and ] (for).<ref name="Ceci & Williams 2009">{{harvnb|Ceci|Williams|2009}}</ref><ref name="Rose 2009">{{harvnb|Rose|2009|pp=786–88}}</ref> The ] of research on race and intelligence has long been a subject of debate: in a 1996 report of the ];{{sfn|Neisser|Boodoo|Bouchard|Boykin|1996}} in guidelines proposed by Gray and Thompson and by Hunt and Carlson;<ref name="Hunt & Carlson 2007"/><ref>{{harvnb|Gray|Thompson|2004}}</ref> and in two editorials in ] in 2009 by ] and by ] and ].<ref name="Ceci & Williams 2009">{{harvnb|Ceci|Williams|2009}}</ref><ref name="Rose 2009">{{Cite journal |last=Rose |first=Steven |date=2009 |title=Should scientists study race and IQ? NO: Science and society do not benefit |url=https://rdcu.be/dj5uC |journal=Nature |volume=457 |issue=7231 |pages=786–788 |doi=10.1038/457786a |pmid=19212384 |bibcode=2009Natur.457..786R |s2cid=42846614 |url-access=limited}}</ref>


According to critics, research on group differences in IQ will reproduce the negative effects of social ideologies (such as ] or ]) that were justified in part on claimed hereditary racial differences.<ref name="AAA"/><ref name="AAA 1994">{{cite web |last=American Anthropological Association |title=Statement on "Race" and Intelligence |year=1994 |url=http://www.aaanet.org/stmts/race.htm |accessdate=March 31, 2010 |ref=harv |postscript=<!--None-->}}</ref> ] maintains that the history of ] makes this field of research difficult to reconcile with current ethical standards for science.<ref name="Rose 2009"/> ] maintains that the history of ] makes this field of research difficult to reconcile with current ethical standards for science.<ref name="Rose 2009"/>
On the other hand, ] has argued that had there been a ban on research on possibly poorly conceived ideas, much valuable research on intelligence testing (including his own discovery of the ]) would not have occurred.<ref>{{harvnb|Flynn|2009b}}</ref>


Many have argued for increased interventions in order to close the gaps.<ref name="Brookings">{{cite web |last1=Jencks |first1=Christopher |last2=Phillips |first2=Meredith |title=The Black-White Test Score Gap |url=https://www.nytimes.com/books/first/j/jencks-gap.html |website=New York Times |access-date=2 October 2016 |archive-date=8 October 2016 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20161008015238/https://www.nytimes.com/books/first/j/jencks-gap.html |url-status=live }}</ref> Flynn writes that "America will have to address all the aspects of black experience that are disadvantageous, beginning with the regeneration of inner city neighborhoods and their schools."<ref name="Flynn 2008">{{harvnb|Flynn|2008}}</ref> Especially in developing nations, society has been urged to take on the prevention of cognitive impairment in children as a high priority. Possible preventable causes include ], ] such as ], ], cerebral ], ] ] and ], newborn ], ], head injuries, ] and ].<ref name="Olness 2003">{{harvnb|Olness|2003}}</ref>
] argues that suggestion of higher ethical standards for research into group differences in intelligence is a ] applied in order to undermine disliked results.<ref name="Gottfredson 2007">{{harvnb|Gottfredson|2007}}</ref> ] has argued that had there been a ban on research on possibly poorly conceived ideas, much valuable research on intelligence testing (including his own discovery of the ]) would not have occurred.<ref>{{harvnb|Flynn|2009}}</ref>

Jensen and Rushton argued that the existence of biological group differences does not rule out, but raises questions about the worthiness of policies such as ] or placing a premium on diversity. They also argued for the importance of teaching people not to overgeneralize or ] individuals based on average group differences, because of the significant overlap of people with varying intelligence between different races.<ref name="Rushton & Jensen 2005"/>

Some who hold the environmentalist viewpoint argue for increased interventions in order to close the gaps.<ref name="Brookings">{{cite web|last1=Jencks|first1=Christopher|last2=Phillips|first2=Meredith|title=The Black-White Test Score Gap|url=https://www.nytimes.com/books/first/j/jencks-gap.html|website=New York Times|publisher=Brookings Institution Press|accessdate=2 October 2016}}</ref> Nisbett argues that schools can be greatly improved and that many interventions at every age level are possible.<ref>{{harvnb|Nisbett|2009}}<!--page number needed (I have the source and should be able to find the page number) --></ref> Flynn, arguing for the importance of the black subculture, writes that "America will have to address all the aspects of black experience that are disadvantageous, beginning with the regeneration of inner city neighbourhoods and their schools. A resident police office and teacher in every apartment block would be a good start."<ref name="Flynn 2008">{{harvnb|Flynn|2008}}</ref> Researchers from both sides agree that interventions should be better researched.<ref name="Nisbett 2009"/><!--page number needed (other references are also available, as in a section above in this article) --><ref name="Rushton & Jensen 2010"/>

Especially in developing nations, society has been urged to take on the prevention of cognitive impairment in children as of the highest priority. Possible preventable causes include ], ] such as ], ], cerebral ], ] ] and ] exposure, newborn ], ], head injuries, ] and ].<ref name="Olness 2003">{{harvnb|Olness|2003}}</ref>


==See also== ==See also==
*] * ]
*] * ]
* ]
*]
*] * ]
* ]
*]


==References== == References ==


=== Notes === === Notes ===
{{notelist}}

=== Citations ===
{{Reflist}} {{Reflist}}


=== Bibliography === === Bibliography ===
{{Refbegin}} {{Refbegin|30em|indent=yes}}
*{{Cite book |last=Alland |first=Alexander Jr |url=https://archive.org/details/raceinmindraceiq00alla |title=Race in Mind |publisher=Palgrave Macmillan |year=2002 |pages= |isbn=978-0-312-23838-4 |url-access=registration }}
<!--Alland 2002-->
*{{Cite web |url=http://www.aaanet.org/stmts/race.htm |title=American Anthropological Association Statement on 'Race' and Intelligence |date=December 1994 |publisher=American Anthropological Association |ref={{harvid|AAA|1994}} |access-date=2005-03-24 |archive-date=2005-03-05 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20050305114339/http://www.aaanet.org/stmts/race.htm |url-status=live }}
*{{cite book |last=Alland |first= Alexander, Jr |title=Race in Mind |url=https://archive.org/details/raceinmindraceiq00alla |url-access=registration |year=2002 |publisher=Palgrave Macmillan |pages= |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite web |url=http://www.aaanet.org/stmts/racepp.htm |title=American Anthropological Association Statement on 'Race' |date=17 May 1998 |publisher=American Anthropological Association |ref={{harvid|AAA|1998}} |access-date=24 February 2011 |archive-date=27 June 2013 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130627184228/http://www.aaanet.org/stmts/racepp.htm |url-status=live }}
<!--AAA 1994-->
*{{Cite journal |year=1996 |title=AAPA Statement on Biological Aspects of Race |url=http://www.physanth.org/association/position-statements/biological-aspects-of-race/?searchterm=race |journal=American Journal of Physical Anthropology |volume=101 |issue=4 |pages=569–570 |doi=10.1002/ajpa.1331010408 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110928030510/http://www.physanth.org/association/position-statements/biological-aspects-of-race/?searchterm=race |archive-date=2011-09-28 |access-date=2011-05-24 |ref={{harvid|AAPA|1996}} }}
*{{cite web |publisher=American Anthropological Association |date=December 1994 |url=http://www.aaanet.org/stmts/race.htm |title=American Anthropological Association Statement on 'Race' and Intelligence |ref={{harvid|AAA|1994}} }}
*{{Cite book |last1=Aronson |first1=E |title=Social Psychology |last2=Wilson |first2=TD |last3=Akert |first3=AM |publisher=Prentice Hall |year=2005 |isbn=978-0-13-178686-8 |edition=5th |location=Upper Saddle River, NJ}}
<!--AAA 1998-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Behrman |first1=JR |last2=Alderman |first2=H |last3=Hoddinott |first3=J |year=2004 |title=Hunger and Malnutrition |url=http://www.copenhagenconsensus.com/Files/Filer/CC/Papers/sammendrag/Accepted__Hunger_summary_070504.pdf |journal=Copenhagen Consensus |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20061025122932/http://www.copenhagenconsensus.com/Files/Filer/CC/Papers/sammendrag/Accepted__Hunger_summary_070504.pdf |archive-date=2006-10-25 }}
*{{cite web |publisher=American Anthropological Association |date=17 May 1998 |url=http://www.aaanet.org/stmts/racepp.htm |title=American Anthropological Association Statement on 'Race' |ref={{harvid|AAA|1998}} }}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Bellinger |first1=David C |last2=Stiles |first2=Karen M |last3=Needleman |first3=Herbert L |date=December 1992 |title=Low-Level Lead Exposure, Intelligence and Academic Achievement: A Long-term Follow-up Study |url=http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/cgi/content/abstract/90/6/855 |journal=Pediatrics |volume=90 |issue=6 |pages=855–61 |doi=10.1542/peds.90.6.855 |pmid=1437425 |s2cid=25746146 |author-link1=David Bellinger |author-link3=Herbert Needleman |access-date=2009-12-19 |archive-date=2019-01-04 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20190104021959/http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/90/6/855?sso=1&sso_redirect_count=1&nfstatus=401&nftoken=00000000-0000-0000-0000-000000000000&nfstatusdescription=ERROR%3A+No+local+token |url-status=live }}
<!--AAPA 1996-->
*{{cite journal |journal=American Journal of Physical Anthropology |volume=101 |issue=4 |pages=569–570 |year=1996 |url=http://www.physanth.org/association/position-statements/biological-aspects-of-race/?searchterm=race |title=AAPA Statement on Biological Aspects of Race |ref={{harvid|AAPA|1996}} |doi=10.1002/ajpa.1331010408 |access-date=2011-05-24 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110928030510/http://www.physanth.org/association/position-statements/biological-aspects-of-race/?searchterm=race |archive-date=2011-09-28 |url-status=dead }} *{{Cite journal |last=Berlet |first=Chip |date=Summer 2003 |title=Into the Mainstream |url=http://www.splcenter.org/intel/intelreport/article.jsp?aid=50 |journal=Intelligence Report |issue=110 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20100202152214/http://www.splcenter.org/intel/intelreport/article.jsp?aid=50 |archive-date=2 February 2010 |access-date=21 February 2012 |df=dmy-all |author-link=Chip Berlet }}
*{{Cite book |last=Block |first=Ned |title=Race and Intelligence: Separating Science from Myth |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates |year=2002 |editor-last=Fish |editor-first=Jefferson |chapter=How heritability misleads about race}}
<!--Aronson, Wilson & Akert 2005-->
*{{Cite journal |last=Borsboom |first=Denny |date=September 2006 |title=The attack of the psychometricians |journal=Psychometrika |volume=71 |issue=3 |pages=425–40 |doi=10.1007/s11336-006-1447-6 |pmc=2779444 |pmid=19946599}}
*{{cite book |last=Aronson |first=E |last2=Wilson |first2=TD |last3=Akert |first3=AM |year=2005 |title=Social Psychology |edition=5th |location=Upper Saddle River, NJ |publisher=Prentice Hall |isbn=978-0-13-178686-8 |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last=Brace |first=C Loring |year=1999 |title=An Anthropological Perspective on 'Race' and Intelligence: The non-clinal nature of human cognitive capabilities |journal=Journal of Anthropological Research |volume=55 |issue=2 |pages=245–64 |doi=10.1086/jar.55.2.3631210 |jstor=3631210 |s2cid=83112950 |author-link=C. Loring Brace}}
<!--Behrman, Alderman & Hoddincott 2004-->
*{{Cite book |last=Brace |first=C. Loring |title=Race is a four letter word |publisher=Oxford University Press |year=2005 |isbn=978-0-19-517351-2 |page=326}}
*{{cite journal |last1=Behrman |first1=JR |last2=Alderman |first2=H |last3=Hoddinott |first3=J |url=http://www.copenhagenconsensus.com/Files/Filer/CC/Papers/sammendrag/Accepted__Hunger_summary_070504.pdf |title=Hunger and Malnutrition |journal=Copenhagen Consensus |year=2004 |ref=harv |url-status=dead |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20061025122932/http://www.copenhagenconsensus.com/Files/Filer/CC/Papers/sammendrag/Accepted__Hunger_summary_070504.pdf |archivedate=2006-10-25 }}
*{{Cite encyclopedia |year=2012 |title=race |encyclopedia=Encyclopædia Britannica |publisher=Encyclopædia Britannica Inc |url=http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/488030/race |edition=Online |ref={{harvid|Britannica|2012}} |access-date=2011-02-24 |archive-date=2011-06-29 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110629075541/http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/488030/race |url-status=live }}
<!--Bellinger, Stiles & Needleman 1992-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Brown |first1=Robert T |last2=Reynolds |first2=Cecil R |last3=Whitaker |first3=Jean S |year=1999 |title=Bias in Mental Testing since "Bias in Mental Testing" |journal=School Psychology Quarterly |volume=14 |issue=3 |pages=208–38 |doi=10.1037/h0089007|s2cid=46561407 }}
*{{cite journal |url=http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/cgi/content/abstract/90/6/855 |title=Low-Level Lead Exposure, Intelligence and Academic Achievement: A Long-term Follow-up Study |first=David C |last=Bellinger |first2=Karen M |last2=Stiles |first3=Herbert L |last3=Needleman |journal=Pediatrics |volume=90 |issue=6 |date=December 1992 |pages=855–61 |ref=harv | pmid = 1437425 }}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Bryc |first1=Katarzyna |last2=Auton |first2=Adam |last3=Nelson |first3=Matthew R. |last4=Oksenberg |first4=Jorge R. |last5=Hauser |first5=Stephen L. |last6=Williams |first6=Scott |last7=Froment |first7=Alain |last8=Bodo |first8=Jean-Marie |last9=Wambebe |first9=Charles |last10=Tishkoff |first10=Sarah A. |last11=Bustamante |first11=Carlos D. |year=2009 |title=Genome-wide patterns of population structure and admixture in West Africans and African Americans |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America |volume=107 |issue=2 |pages=786–91 |bibcode=2010PNAS..107..786B |doi=10.1073/pnas.0909559107 |pmc=2818934 |pmid=20080753 |ref={{harvid|Bryc et al.|2009}} |doi-access=free}}
<!--Berlet 2003-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Campbell |first1=FA |last2=Ramey |first2=CT |year=1994 |title=Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic achievement: A follow-up study of children from low-income families |journal=Child Development |volume=65 |issue=2 |pages=684–698 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00777.x}}
*{{cite journal |url=http://www.splcenter.org/intel/intelreport/article.jsp?aid=50 |title=Into the Mainstream |accessdate=21 February 2012 |last=Berlet |first=Chip |date=Summer 2003 |issue=110 |journal=Intelligence Report |ref={{harvid|Berlet|2003}} |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20100202152214/http://www.splcenter.org/intel/intelreport/article.jsp?aid=50 |archive-date=2 February 2010 |url-status=dead |df=dmy-all }}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Campbell |first1=Frances A |last2=Ramey |first2=Craig T |last3=Pungello |first3=Elizabeth |last4=Sparling |first4=Joseph |last5=Miller-Johnson |first5=Shari |year=2002 |title=Early Childhood Education: Young Adult Outcomes From the Abecedarian Project |journal=Applied Developmental Science |volume=6 |pages=42–57 |doi=10.1207/s1532480xads0601_05 |s2cid=71602425 |ref={{harvid|Campbell et al.|2002}}}}
<!--Block 2002 -->
*{{Cite book |last1=Carson |first1=Michael |title='Race', IQ and Genes |last2=Beckwith |first2=Jon |date=2001 |publisher=John Wiley & Sons, Ltd |isbn=978-0-470-01590-2 |pages=1–5 |language=en |doi=10.1002/9780470015902.a0005689.pub3}}
*{{cite book |last=Block |first=Ned |year=2002 |chapter=How heritability misleads about race |editor-last= Fish |editor-first=Jefferson |title=Race and Intelligence: Separating Science from Myth |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates |ref=harv }}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Ceci |first1=SJ |last2=Williams |first2=WM |year=2009 |title=Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? Yes: the scientific truth must be pursued |journal=Nature |volume=457 |issue=7231 |pages=788–9 |bibcode=2009Natur.457..788C |doi=10.1038/457788a |pmid=19212385 |s2cid=205044224 |author-link1=Stephen J. Ceci |author-link2=Wendy M. Williams |doi-access=free}}
<!--Boorsham 2006-->
*{{cite journal |title=The attack of the psychometricians |first=Denny |last=Borsboom |journal=Psychometrika |volume= 71 |issue=3 |pages=425–40 |date=September 2006 |doi=10.1007/s11336-006-1447-6 |ref=harv |pmc=2779444 |pmid=19946599}} *{{Cite journal |last=Cooper |first=R. S. |year=2005 |title=Race and IQ: Molecular Genetics as Deus ex Machina |journal=American Psychologist |volume=60 |issue=1 |pages=71–76 |citeseerx=10.1.1.624.5059 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.71 |pmid=15641923}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Cottrell |first1=Jonathan M. |last2=Newman |first2=Daniel A. |last3=Roisman |first3=Glenn I. |date=November 2015 |title=Explaining the black-white gap in cognitive test scores: Toward a theory of adverse impact |journal=The Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=100 |issue=6 |pages=1713–1736 |doi=10.1037/apl0000020 |issn=1939-1854 |pmid=25867168}}
<!--Brace 1999 -->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Cronshaw |first1=Steven F |last2=Hamilton |first2=Leah K |last3=Onyura |first3=Betty R |last4=Winston |first4=Andrew S |date=September 2006 |title=Case for Non-Biased Intelligence Testing Against Black Africans Has Not Been Made: A Comment on Rushton, Skuy, and Bons |journal=International Journal of Selection and Assessment |volume=14 |issue=3 |pages=278–287 |doi=10.1111/j.1468-2389.2006.00346.x |s2cid=91179275 |ref={{harvid|Cronshaw et al.|2006}}}}
*{{cite journal | last=Brace | first=C Loring | year=1999 | title=An Anthropological Perspective on 'Race' and Intelligence: The non-clinal nature of human cognitive capabilities | journal=Journal of Anthropological Research | volume=55 | jstor=3631210 | pages=245–64 | issue=2 |ref=harv | doi=10.1086/jar.55.2.3631210 }}
*{{Cite book |last1=Daley |first1=C. E. |title=The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence |last2=Onwuegbuzie |first2=A. J. |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=2011 |isbn=978-0-521-51806-2 |editor-last=Sternberg |editor-first=R. |location=Cambridge New York |pages=293–306 |chapter=Race and Intelligence |editor-last2=Kaufman |editor-first2=S. B.}}
<!--Brace 2005 -->
*{{cite book |last=Brace |first= C. Loring |year=2005 |title= Race is a four letter word |publisher= Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0-19-517351-2 |page=326 |ref=harv}} *{{Cite book |last=Deary |first=Ian J |title=Intelligence: A Very Short Introduction |publisher=Oxford University Press |year=2001 |isbn=978-0-19-289321-5}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Deary |first1=I. J. |last2=Penke |first2=L. |last3=Johnson |first3=W. |year=2010 |title=The neuroscience of human intelligence differences |url=https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/8895819/the_neuroscience_of_human_intelligence.pdf |journal=Nature Reviews Neuroscience |volume=11 |issue=3 |pages=201–211 |doi=10.1038/nrn2793 |pmid=20145623 |s2cid=5136934 |hdl=20.500.11820/9b11fac3-47d0-424c-9d1c-fe6f9ff2ecac |hdl-access=free |access-date=2019-01-26 |archive-date=2018-07-19 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180719195613/https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/8895819/the_neuroscience_of_human_intelligence.pdf |url-status=live }}
<!--Britannica 2012-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Deary |first1=IJ |last2=Johnson |first2=W |last3=Houlihan |first3=LM |year=2009 |title=Genetic foundations of human intelligence |url=https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/8895353/genetic_foundations_of_human_intelligence.pdf |journal=Human Genetics |volume=126 |issue=1 |pages=215–32 |doi=10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4 |pmid=19294424 |hdl=20.500.11820/c3e0a75b-dad6-4860-91c6-b242221af681 |s2cid=4975607 |hdl-access=free |access-date=2019-12-07 |archive-date=2020-08-01 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200801215610/https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/8895353/genetic_foundations_of_human_intelligence.pdf |url-status=live }}
*{{cite encyclopedia |year=2012 |title=race |encyclopedia=Encyclopædia Britannica |publisher=Encyclopædia Britannica Inc |edition=Online |url=http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/488030/race |ref={{harvid|Britannica|2012}} }}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Dickens |first1=William T |last2=Flynn |first2=James R |year=2001 |title=Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved |journal=Psychological Review |volume=108 |issue=2 |pages=346–69 |citeseerx=10.1.1.139.2436 |doi=10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346 |pmid=11381833 |author-link1=William T. Dickens}}
<!--Brown 1999-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Dickens |first1=William T |last2=Flynn |first2=James R |year=2006 |title=Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence from Standardization Samples |url=http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_iq.pdf |journal=Psychological Science |volume=17 |issue=10 |pages=913–20 |citeseerx=10.1.1.186.2540 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01802.x |pmid=17100793 |s2cid=6593169 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20091009095003/http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_IQ.pdf |archive-date=2009-10-09 }}
*{{cite journal | doi = 10.1037/h0089007 |last = Brown |first= Robert T |last2=Reynolds |first2=Cecil R |last3=Whitaker |first3=Jean S | year = 1999 | title = Bias in Mental Testing since "Bias in Mental Testing" | url = | journal = School Psychology Quarterly | volume = 14 | issue = 3| pages = 208–38 |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Elhoweris |first1=Hala |last2=Mutua |first2=Kagendo |last3=Alsheikh |first3=Negmeldin |last4=Holloway |first4=Pauline |year=2005 |title=Effect of Children's Ethnicity on Teachers' Referral and Recommendation Decisions in Gifted and Talented Programs |journal=Remedial and Special Education |volume=26 |pages=25–31 |doi=10.1177/07419325050260010401 |s2cid=146321193 |ref={{harvid|Elhoweris et al.|2005}}}}
<!--Bryc et al. 2009-->
*{{Cite journal |last=Eppig |first=Christopher |year=2011 |title=Why Is Average IQ Higher in Some Places? |url=http://www.scientificamerican.com/article.cfm?id=why-is-average-iq-higher-in-some-places |journal=Scientific American |access-date=2011-11-20 |archive-date=2020-04-01 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20200401001619/http://www.scientificamerican.com/article/why-is-average-iq-higher-in-some-places/ |url-status=live }}
*{{cite journal |year=2009 |first1=Katarzyna |last1=Bryc |first2=Adam |last2=Auton |first3=Matthew R. |last3=Nelson |first4=Jorge R. |last4=Oksenberg |first5=Stephen L. |last5=Hauser |first6=Scott |last6=Williams |first7=Alain |last7=Froment |first8=Jean-Marie |last8=Bodo |first9=Charles |last9=Wambebe |first10=Sarah A. |last10=Tishkoff |first11=Carlos D. |last11=Bustamante |title=Genome-wide patterns of population structure and admixture in West Africans and African Americans |journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America |doi=10.1073/pnas.0909559107 |ref={{harvid|Bryc et al.|2009}} |pmid=20080753 |pmc=2818934 |volume=107 |issue=2 |pages=786–91|bibcode=2010PNAS..107..786B }}
*{{Cite book |last=Evans |first=Gavin |title=Skin Deep: Journeys in the Divisive Science of Race |date=2019 |isbn=978-1-78607-622-9 |location=London |oclc=1059232398 |publisher=]}}
<!--Campbell & Ramey 1994-->
*{{Cite journal |last=Eyferth |first=K |year=1961 |title=Leistungern verscheidener Gruppen von Besatzungskindern Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest für Kinder (HAWIK) |journal=Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie |language=de |volume=113 |pages=222–41}}
*{{cite journal |last=Campbell |first=FA |last2=Ramey |first2=CT |year=1994 |title=Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic achievement: A follow-up study of children from low-income families |journal=Child Development |volume=65 |issue=2 |pages=684–698 |ref=harv |doi=10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00777.x}}
*{{Cite book |last=Eysenck |first=Hans J |title=Arthur Jensen: Consensus and controversy |publisher=Falmer |year=1987 |editor-last=Modgil |editor-first=S |location=New York, NY |chapter=Intelligence and Reaction Time: The Contribution of Arthur Jensen |editor-last2=Modgil |editor-first2=C |author-link=Hans Eysenck}}
<!--Campbell et al. 2002 -->
*{{Cite book |last=Falk |first=Avner |title=Anti-semitism: a history and psychoanalysis of contemporary hatred |publisher=Praeger |year=2008 |isbn=978-0-313-35385-7 |page=312}}
*{{cite journal |first=Frances A |last=Campbell |first2=Craig T |last2=Ramey |first3=Elizabeth |last3=Pungello |first4=Joseph |last4=Sparling |first5=Shari |last5=Miller-Johnson |title=Early Childhood Education: Young Adult Outcomes From the Abecedarian Project |journal=Applied Developmental Science |volume= 6 |year= 2002 |pages= 42–57 |ref={{harvid|Campbell et al.|2002}} |doi=10.1207/s1532480xads0601_05}}
*{{Cite journal |last=Flynn |first=James R |year=1991 |title=Reaction times show that both Chinese and British children are more intelligent than one another |journal=Perceptual and Motor Skills |volume=72 |issue=2 |pages=544–6 |doi=10.2466/pms.1991.72.2.544 |s2cid=144091744 |author-link=James Flynn (academic)}}
<!--Carson & Beckwith 2001-->
*{{Cite journal |last=Flynn |first=James R |date=3 September 2008 |title=Perspectives: Still a question of black vs white? |url=http://www.cambridgeblog.org/2008/09/james-flynn-in-the-new-scientist/ |journal=New Scientist |edition=magazine issue |issue=2672 |doi=10.1016/S0262-4079(08)62253-8 |volume=199 |pages=48–50 |access-date=6 February 2011 |archive-date=7 July 2011 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110707111349/http://www.cambridgeblog.org/2008/09/james-flynn-in-the-new-scientist/ |url-status=live }}
*{{Cite book |last=Carson |first=Michael |last2=Beckwith |first2=Jon |date=2001 |title='Race', IQ and Genes |language=en |publisher=John Wiley & Sons, Ltd |doi=10.1002/9780470015902.a0005689.pub3 |isbn=9780470015902|ref={{harvid|Carson|Beckwith|2001}} |pages=1–5}}
*{{Cite journal |last=Flynn |first=James R |year=2009b |title=Would you wish the research undone? |journal=Nature |volume=458 |issue=7235 |page=146 |bibcode=2009Natur.458..146F |doi=10.1038/458146a |pmid=19279612 |doi-access=free}}
<!--Ceci 2009-->
*{{Cite journal |last=Flynn |first=James R |year=2010 |title=The spectacles through which I see the race and IQ debate |url=http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/flynn2010a.pdf |journal=Intelligence |volume=38 |issue=4 |pages=363–6 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2010.05.001 |access-date=2011-02-18 |archive-date=2020-12-07 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20201207224050/http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/flynn2010a.pdf |url-status=live }}
*{{cite journal |title=Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? Yes: the scientific truth must be pursued |last=Ceci |first=SJ |last2=Williams |first2=WM |year=2009 |volume=457 |pages=788–9 |journal=Nature |doi=10.1038/457788a |issue=7231|bibcode = 2009Natur.457..788C |pmid=19212385 |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite book |last=Flynn |first=James R. |title=Are We Getting Smarter? Rising IQ in the Twenty-First Century |publisher=] |year=2012 |isbn=978-1-107-60917-4 |location=Cambridge |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/arewegettingsmar0000flyn }}
<!--Cooper 2005-->
**{{lay source |template=cite periodical |author=Lea Winerman |date=March 2013 |title=Smarter than ever? |volume=44 |issue=3 |url=http://www.apa.org/monitor/2013/03/smarter.aspx |periodical=Monitor on Psychology}}
*{{Cite journal | last1 = Cooper | first1 = R. S. | title = Race and IQ: Molecular Genetics as Deus ex Machina | doi = 10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.71 | journal = American Psychologist | volume = 60 | issue = 1 | pages = 71–76 | year = 2005 | pmid = 15641923| ref = harv| citeseerx = 10.1.1.624.5059 }}
*{{Cite journal |last=Gottfredson |first=Linda S |year=1997 |title=Mainstream Science on Intelligence (editorial) |url=http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/1997mainstream.pdf |journal=Intelligence |volume=24 |pages=13–23 |doi=10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90011-8 |author-link=Linda Gottfredson |access-date=2005-01-08 |archive-date=2014-12-22 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20141222052006/http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/1997mainstream.pdf |url-status=live }}
<!--Cottrell, Newman & Roisman 2015-->
*{{Cite journal |last=Cottrell |first=Jonathan M. |last2=Newman |first2=Daniel A. |last3=Roisman |first3=Glenn I. |date=November 2015 |title=Explaining the black-white gap in cognitive test scores: Toward a theory of adverse impact |journal=The Journal of Applied Psychology |volume=100 |issue=6 |pages=1713–1736 |doi=10.1037/apl0000020 |issn=1939-1854 |pmid=25867168|ref={{harvid|Cottrell|Newman|Roisman|2015}}}} *{{Cite journal |last=Gottfredson |first=Linda S |year=2007 |title=Applying Double Standards to 'Divisive' Ideas: Commentary on Hunt and Carlson |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |volume=2 |issue=2 |pages=216–220 |doi=10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00039.x |pmid=26151962 |s2cid=21203607}}
*{{Cite book |last=Gould |first=Stephen Jay |url=https://archive.org/details/mismeasureofman00goulrich |title=The Mismeasure of Man |publisher=Norton |year=1981 |isbn=978-0-393-30056-7 |location=New York, London |url-access=registration |author-link=Stephen Jay Gould }}
<!--Cronshaw et al. 2006-->
*{{Cite book |last=Graves |first=Joseph L. |url=https://archive.org/details/emperorsnewcloth00grav |title=The Emperor's New Clothes: Biological Theories of Race at the Millennium |publisher=Rutgers University Press |year=2001 |isbn=978-0-8135-2847-2 |edition=Kindle |url-access=registration |author-link=Joseph L. Graves Jr. }}
*{{cite journal |title=Case for Non-Biased Intelligence Testing Against Black Africans Has Not Been Made: A Comment on Rushton, Skuy, and Bons |first=Steven F |last=Cronshaw |first2=Leah K |last2=Hamilton |first3=Betty R |last3=Onyura |first4=Andrew S |last4=Winston |journal=International Journal of Selection and Assessment |volume=14 |issue=3 |pages=278–287 |date= September 2006 |doi=10.1111/j.1468-2389.2006.00346.x |ref={{harvid|Cronshaw et al.|2006}} }}
*{{Cite book |last=Graves |first=Joseph L |title=Race and Intelligence: Separating Myth from Reality |publisher=Laurence Erlbaum Associates |year=2002a |isbn=978-0-8058-3757-5 |editor-last=Fish |editor-first=Jefferson M |pages=57–94 |chapter=The Misuse of Life History Theory: JP Rushton and the Pseudoscience of Racial Hierarchy}}
<!-- Daley & Onwuegbuzie 2011 -->
*{{Cite journal |last=Graves |first=Joseph L Jr |year=2002b |title=What a tangled web he weaves: Race, reproductive strategies and Rushton's life history theory |journal=Anthropological Theory |volume=2 |issue=2 |pages=131–54 |doi=10.1177/1469962002002002627 |s2cid=144377864}}
*{{cite book |last1=Daley| first1 = C. E. |last2=Onwuegbuzie| first2 = A. J. | editor1-last = Sternberg | editor1-first = R. | editor2-last = Kaufman | editor2-first = S. B. | title = The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence | chapter = Race and Intelligence | pages = 293–306| publisher = Cambridge University Press | location = Cambridge New York | year = 2011 | isbn = 9780521518062 |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Gray |first1=Jeremy R |last2=Thompson |first2=Paul M |year=2004 |title=Neurobiology of intelligence: science and ethics |url=https://www.mun.ca/biology/scarr/Gray_%26_Thompson_2004_Nature_Rev_Neurosci_5%2C471-482.pdf |journal=Nature Reviews Neuroscience |volume=5 |issue=6 |pages=471–82 |doi=10.1038/nrn1405 |pmid=15152197 |s2cid=2430677 |access-date=2019-05-10 |archive-date=2018-07-14 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20180714065640/http://www.mun.ca/biology/scarr/Gray_%26_Thompson_2004_Nature_Rev_Neurosci_5%2C471-482.pdf |url-status=live }}
<!--Deary 2001-->
*{{Cite book |last1=Grossman |first1=James B |title=The evolution of intelligence |last2=Kaufman |publisher=Routledge |year=2001 |isbn=978-0-8058-3267-9 |editor-last=Sternberg |editor-first=Robert J |chapter=Evolutionary Psychology: Promise and Perils |editor-last2=Kaufman |editor-first2=James C}}
*{{cite book |first=Ian J |last=Deary |title=Intelligence: A Very Short Introduction |year=2001 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0-19-289321-5 |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Hampshire |first1=A. |last2=Highfield |first2=R. R. |last3=Parkin |first3=B. L. |last4=Owen |first4=A. M. |year=2012 |title=Fractionating human intelligence |journal=Neuron |volume=76 |pages=1225–1237 |doi=10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.022 |pmid=23259956 |number=6 |doi-access=free}}
<!--Deary, Penke & Johnson 2010 -->
*{{Cite book |last1=Herrnstein |first1=Richard J |url=https://archive.org/details/bellcurveintelli00herr |title=The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life |last2=Murray |first2=Charles |publisher=Free Press |year=1994 |isbn=978-0-02-914673-6 |location=New York |url-access=registration |author-link1=Richard Herrnstein |author-link2=Charles Murray (political scientist) }}
*{{cite journal|last1=Deary|first1=I. J.|last2=Penke|first2=L.|last3=Johnson|first3=W.|year=2010|title=The neuroscience of human intelligence differences|journal=Nature Reviews Neuroscience|volume=11|issue=3|pages=201–211|ref=harv|doi=10.1038/nrn2793|pmid=20145623|url=https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/8895819/the_neuroscience_of_human_intelligence.pdf}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Ho |first1=KC |last2=Roessmann |first2=U |last3=Straumfjord |first3=JV |last4=Monroe |first4=G |date=December 1980 |title=Analysis of brain weight. I. Adult brain weight in relation to sex, race, and age. |journal=Archives of Pathology & Laboratory Medicine |volume=104 |issue=12 |pages=635–9 |pmid=6893659 |ref={{harvid|Ho et al.|1980}}}}
<!--Deary, Johnson & Houlihan 2009 -->
*{{Cite book |last=Hunt |first=Earl |title=Human Intelligence |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=2010 |isbn=978-0-521-70781-7 |author-link=Earl B. Hunt}}
*{{cite journal |year=2009 |pages=215–32 |issue=1 |volume=126 |title=Genetic foundations of human intelligence |first=IJ |last=Deary |first2=W |last2=Johnson |first3=LM |last3=Houlihan |pmid=19294424 |journal= Human Genetics |doi=10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4 |ref=harv|url=https://www.pure.ed.ac.uk/ws/files/8895353/genetic_foundations_of_human_intelligence.pdf }}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Hunt |first1=Earl |last2=Carlson |first2=Jerry |year=2007 |title=Considerations relating to the study of group differences in intelligence |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |volume=2 |issue=2 |pages=194–213 |doi=10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00037.x |pmid=26151960 |s2cid=32540018}}
<!--Dick et al. 2007 -->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Hunt |first1=Earl |last2=Wittmann |first2=Werner |date=January–February 2008 |title=National intelligence and national prosperity |journal=Intelligence |volume=36 |issue=1 |pages=1–9 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.11.002}}
*{{cite journal | doi = 10.1007/s10519-006-9131-2 |last1=Danielle M. |first1=Dick |last2=Fazil |first2=Aliev |last3=John |first3=Kramer |last4=Jen C. |first4=Wang |last5=Anthony |first5=Hinrichs |last6=Sarah |first6=Bertelsen |last7=Sam |first7=Kuperman |last8=Marc |first8=Schuckit |last9=John, Jr |first9=Nurnberger |last10=Howard J. |first10=Edenberg |last11=Bernice |first11=Porjesz |last12=Henri |first12=Begleiter |last13=Victor |first13=Hesselbrock |last14=Alison |first14=Goate |last15=Laura |first15=Bierut | year = 2007 | title = Association of CHRM2 with IQ: converging evidence for a gene influencing intelligence | journal = Behavioral Genetics | volume = 37 | issue = 2| pages = 265–72 | pmid=17160701 |ref={{harvid|Dick et al.|2007}} }}
*{{Cite journal |last=Irvine |first=SH |year=1983 |title=Where intelligence tests fail |journal=Nature |volume=302 |issue=5907 |page=371 |bibcode=1983Natur.302..371I |doi=10.1038/302371b0 |s2cid=41604729 |doi-access=free}}
<!--Dickens & Flynn 2001 -->
*{{Cite book |url=https://archive.org/details/humanabilitiesin0000unse |title=Human Abilities in Cultural Context |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=1988 |isbn=978-0-521-34482-1 |editor-last=Irvine |editor-first=SH |editor-last2=Berry |editor-first2=JW }}
*{{cite journal |doi=10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.346 |last=Dickens |first=William T |last2=Flynn |first2=James R |title=Heritability estimates versus large environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved |journal=Psychological Review |volume=108 |issue=2 |pages=346–69 |year=2001 |pmid=11381833 |ref=harv |citeseerx=10.1.1.139.2436 }}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Ivanovic |first1=Daniza M. |last2=Leiva |first2=Boris P. |last3=Pérez |first3=Hernán T. |last4=Olivares |first4=Manuel G. |last5=Dı́az |first5=Nora S. |last6=Urruti |first6=Marı́a Soledad C. |last7=Almagià |first7=Atilio F. |last8=Toro |first8=Triana D. |last9=Miller |first9=Patricio T. |last10=Bosch |first10=Enrique O. |last11=Larraı́n |first11=Cristián G. |year=2004 |title=Head size and intelligence, learning, nutritional status and brain development. Head, IQ, learning, nutrition and brain |journal=Neuropsychologia |volume=42 |issue=8 |pages=1118–31 |doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.022 |pmid=15093150 |s2cid=2114185 |ref={{harvid|Ivanovic et al.|2004}}}}
<!--Dickens & Flynn 2006-->
*{{Cite book |last1=Jackson |first1=John P. |title=Race, Racism, and Science: Social Impact and Interaction |last2=Weidman |first2=Nadine M. |publisher=ABC-CLIO |year=2004 |page=23}}
*{{cite journal |last=Dickens |first=William T |last2=Flynn |first2=James R |year=2006 |title=Black Americans Reduce the Racial IQ Gap: Evidence from Standardization Samples |journal=Psychological Science |volume=17 |issue=10 |pages=913–20 |url=http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_iq.pdf |ref=harv |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01802.x |pmid=17100793 |url-status=dead |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20091009095003/http://www.brookings.edu/views/papers/dickens/20060619_IQ.pdf |archivedate=2009-10-09 |citeseerx=10.1.1.186.2540 }}
*{{Cite book |last=Jackson |first=John P. |title=Science for Segregation: Race, Law, and the Case against Brown v. Board of Education |publisher=] |year=2005 |isbn=978-0-8147-4271-6}}
<!--Elhoweris et al. 2005-->
**{{lay source |template=cite web |title=Book Review: Science for Segregation: Race, Law, and the Case Against Brown v. Board of Education |url=http://www.historycooperative.org/journals/lhr/25.2/br_19.html |website=History Cooperative}}
*{{cite journal |title=Effect of Children's Ethnicity on Teachers' Referral and Recommendation Decisions in Gifted and Talented Programs |first3=Negmeldin |last3=Alsheikh |first=Hala |last=Elhoweris |first4=Pauline |last4=Holloway |first2=Kagendo |last2=Mutua |journal= Remedial and Special Education |volume=26 |pages=25–31 |year= 2005 |ref={{harvid|Elhoweris et al.|2005}} |doi=10.1177/07419325050260010401 }}
*{{Cite journal |last=Jensen |first=Arthur R |year=1969 |title=How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement? |journal=Harvard Educational Review |volume=39 |pages=1–123 |doi=10.17763/haer.39.1.l3u15956627424k7 |title-link=How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement? |author-link=Arthur Jensen}}
<!--Eppig, Fincher & Thornhill 2009-->
*{{Cite book |last=Jensen |first=Arthur R |title=Educability and Group Differences |publisher=Methuen |year=1973 |isbn=978-0-06-012194-5 |location=London |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/educabilitygroup0000jens }}
*{{cite journal |first=Christopher |last=Eppig |first2=Corey L |last2=Fincher |first3=Randy |last3= Thornhill |title=Parasite prevalence and the worldwide distribution of cognitive ability |journal=Proceedings of the Royal Society B |year=2010 |doi=10.1098/rspb.2010.0973 |ref=harv |pmid=20591860 |pmc=2992705 |volume=277 |issue=1701 |pages=3801–8}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Jensen |first1=Arthur R |last2=Whang |first2=PA |year=1993 |title=Reaction times and intelligence: a comparison of Chinese-American and Anglo-American children |journal=Journal of Biosocial Science |volume=25 |issue=3 |pages=397–410 |doi=10.1017/s0021932000020721 |pmid=8360233|s2cid=18171436 }}
<!--Eppig 2011-->
*{{Cite book |last=Jensen |first=Arthur R |url=https://archive.org/details/gfactorscienc00jens |title=The g factor: The science of mental ability |publisher=Praeger |year=1998 |isbn=978-0-275-96103-9 |location=Westport, CT }}
*{{cite journal |journal= Scientific American |year=2011 |title= Why Is Average IQ Higher in Some Places? |first= Christopher |last=Eppig|url=http://www.scientificamerican.com/article.cfm?id=why-is-average-iq-higher-in-some-places |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite book |last=Jensen |first=AR |title=Clocking the mind: Mental chronometry and individual differences |publisher=Elsevier |year=2006 |isbn=978-0-08-044939-5 |location=Amsterdam}}
<!--Eyferth 1961-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Jensen |first1=Arthur R |last2=Johnson |first2=Fred W |date=May–June 1994 |title=Race and sex differences in head size and IQ |journal=Intelligence |volume=18 |issue=3 |pages=309–33 |doi=10.1016/0160-2896(94)90032-9}}
*{{cite journal |first=K |last=Eyferth |title=Leistungern verscheidener Gruppen von Besatzungskindern Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest für Kinder (HAWIK) |journal=Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie |volume=113 |pages=222–41 |year=1961 |language=German |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last=Kamin |first=Leon J. |date=March 2006 |title=African IQ and Mental Retardation |journal=South African Journal of Psychology |volume=36 |issue=1 |pages=1–9 |doi=10.1177/008124630603600101 |s2cid=92984213 |author-link=Leon Kamin}}
<!--Eysenck 1987 -->
*{{Cite encyclopedia |last=Kaplan |first=Jonathan Michael |url=http://www.els.net/WileyCDA/ElsArticle/refId-a0005857.html |title='Race': What Biology Can Tell Us about a Social Construct |date=January 2011 |encyclopedia=Encyclopedia of Life Sciences |isbn=978-0-470-01617-6 |doi=10.1002/9780470015902.a0005857 |access-date=2012-02-22 |archive-date=2021-05-25 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20210525023933/http://www.els.net/WileyCDA/ElsArticle/refId-a0005857.html |url-status=live }}
*{{cite book |last=Eysenck |first=Hans J |year= 1987 |chapter=Intelligence and Reaction Time: The Contribution of Arthur Jensen |editor1-first=S |editor1-last= Modgil |editor2-first=C |editor2-last=Modgil |title=Arthur Jensen: Consensus and controversy |location=New York, NY |publisher=Falmer |ref=harv }}
*{{Cite journal |last=Kaplan |first=Jonathan Michael |date=6 February 2014 |title=Race, IQ, and the search for statistical signals associated with so-called "X"-factors: environments, racism, and the "hereditarian hypothesis" |journal=Biology & Philosophy |volume=30 |issue=1 |pages=1–17 |doi=10.1007/s10539-014-9428-0 |s2cid=85351431}}
<!--Falk 2008-->
* {{cite journal |last1=Kaplan |first1=J. M. |last2=Winther |first2=R. G. |title=Prisoners of Abstraction? The Theory and Measure of Genetic Variation, and the Very Concept of 'Race' |journal=] |date=2013 |volume=7 |pages=401–412 |doi=10.1002/9780470015902.a0005857 |url=http://philpapers.org/archive/KAPPOA.14.pdf}}
*{{cite book |first=Avner |last=Falk |title=Anti-semitism: a history and psychoanalysis of contemporary hatred |publisher=Praeger |year=2008 |page=312 |isbn=978-0-313-35385-7 |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Kaszycka |first1=Katarzyna A. |last2=Štrkalj |first2=Goran |last3=Strzałko |first3=Jan |year=2009 |title=Current Views of European Anthropologists on Race: Influence of Educational and Ideological Background |journal=American Anthropologist |volume=111 |issue=1 |pages=43–56 |doi=10.1111/j.1548-1433.2009.01076.x}}
<!--Flynn 1991-->
*{{cite journal |last=Flynn |first=James R |title=Reaction times show that both Chinese and British children are more intelligent than one another |journal=Perceptual and Motor Skills |volume=72 |issue=2 |pages=544–6 |year=1991 |ref=harv |doi=10.2466/pms.1991.72.2.544}} *{{Cite journal |last=Lieberman |first=Leonard |year=2001 |title=How "Caucasoids" got such big crania and why they shrank: from Morton to Rushton." |journal=Current Anthropology |volume=42 |issue=1 |pages=69–95 |doi=10.1086/318434 |pmid=14992214 |s2cid=224794908}}
*{{Cite book |last=Loehlin |first=John C |title=The Handbook of Intelligence |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=2000 |editor-last=Sternberg |editor-first=Robert J |location=Cambridge |chapter=Group Differences in Intelligence}}
<!--Flynn 2008 -->
*{{Cite book |last=Ludy |first=Benjamin T |title=Brief History of Modern Psychology |publisher=Wiley-Blackwell |year=2006 |isbn=978-1-4051-3206-0 |pages=188–91}}
*{{cite journal |title=Perspectives: Still a question of black vs white? |first=James R |last=Flynn |journal= New Scientist |date= 3 September 2008 |edition=magazine issue |issue=2672 |url=http://www.cambridgeblog.org/2008/09/james-flynn-in-the-new-scientist/ |ref=harv }}
*{{Cite journal |last=MacEachern |first=Scott |date=March 2006 |title=Africanist archaeology and ancient IQ: racial science and cultural evolution in the twenty-first century |journal=World Archaeology |volume=38 |issue=1 |pages=72–92 |doi=10.1080/00438240500509918 |s2cid=162340124}}
<!--Flynn 2009-->
*{{Cite journal |last=Flynn |first=James R |journal=Nature |volume=458 |page=146 |year=2009 |title=Would you wish the research undone? |ref=harv |issue=7235 |doi=10.1038/458146a|bibcode = 2009Natur.458..146F |pmid=19279612}} *{{Cite journal |last=Mackenzie |first=Brian |date=1984 |title=Explaining race differences in IQ: The logic, the methodology, and the evidence. |journal=American Psychologist |language=en |volume=39 |issue=11 |pages=1214–1233 |doi=10.1037/0003-066x.39.11.1214}}
*{{Cite book |last=Mackintosh |first=N. J. |url=https://archive.org/details/iqhumanintellige00mack |url-access=registration |title=IQ and Human Intelligence |publisher=Oxford University Press |year=1998 |isbn=978-0-19-852367-3 |author-link=Nicholas Mackintosh }}
<!--Flynn 2010 -->
*{{Cite book |last=Mackintosh |first=N. J. |title=IQ and Human Intelligence |publisher=Oxford University Press |year=2011 |isbn=978-0-19-958559-5 |edition=second |location=Oxford}}
*{{cite journal |title=The spectacles through which I see the race and IQ debate |journal=Intelligence |first=James R |last=Flynn |issue=4 |year=2010 |pages=363–6 |url=http://www.iapsych.com/iqmr/fe/LinkedDocuments/flynn2010a.pdf |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2010.05.001 |ref=harv |volume=38}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Manly |first1=JJ |last2=Byrd |first2=DA |last3=Touradji |first3=P |last4=Stern |first4=Yaakov |year=2004 |title=Acculturation, Reading Level, and Neuropsychological Test Performance Among African American Elders |journal=Applied Neuropsychology |volume=11 |issue=1 |pages=37–46 |doi=10.1207/s15324826an1101_5 |pmid=15471745 |s2cid=30490056 |ref={{harvid|Manly et al.|2004}}}}
<!--Flynn 2012 -->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Manly |first1=Jennifer J |last2=Jacobs |first2=Diane M |last3=Touradji |first3=Pegah |last4=Small |first4=Scott A |last5=Stern |first5=Yaakov |year=2002 |title=Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders |journal=Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society |volume=8 |issue=3 |pages=341–348 |doi=10.1017/S1355617702813157 |pmid=11939693 |s2cid=14360177 |ref={{harvid|Manly et al.|2002}}}}
*{{cite book | last=Flynn | first=James R. | title=Are We Getting Smarter? Rising IQ in the Twenty-First Century |location=Cambridge | publisher=] | isbn=978-1-107-60917-4 | year=2012 | lay-url=http://www.apa.org/monitor/2013/03/smarter.aspx | laydate=16 May 2013 | ref=harv}}
*{{Cite book |last1=Maltby |first1=John |title=Personality, Individual Differences and Intelligence |last2=Day |first2=Liz |last3=Macaskill |first3=Ann |publisher=] |year=2007 |isbn=978-0-13-129760-9 |pages=334–47}}
<!--Gottfredson 1997-->
*{{Cite journal|last=Gottfredson|first= Linda S |year=1997 |title= Mainstream Science on Intelligence (editorial) |journal=Intelligence | volume=24 |pages=13–23 |url=http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/reprints/1997mainstream.pdf |doi=10.1016/S0160-2896(97)90011-8 |ref=harv}} *{{Cite journal |last=McDaniel |first=M. A. |year=2005 |title=Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence |journal=Intelligence |volume=33 |issue=4 |pages=337–346 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2004.11.005}}
*<!--not peer reviewed-->{{Cite journal |last=Mickelson |first=Roslyn Arlin |year=2003 |title=When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? A Social Science Perspective |journal=Teachers College Record |volume=105 |issue=6 |pages=1052–1086 |doi=10.1111/1467-9620.00277}}
<!--Gottfredson 2007-->
*{{Cite book |last=Miele |first=Frank |title=Intelligence, Race and Genetics: Conversations with Arthur R. Jensen |publisher=Basic Books |year=2002 |page=256 |author-link=Frank Miele}}
*{{cite journal |title=Applying Double Standards to 'Divisive' Ideas: Commentary on Hunt and Carlson |first=Linda S |last=Gottfredson |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |year=2007 |volume=2 |issue=2 |pages=216–220 |ref=harv |doi=10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00039.x|pmid=26151962 }}
*{{Cite journal |last=MMWR |date=27 May 2005 |title=Blood Lead Levels—United States, 1999–2002 |url=https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5420a5.htm |journal=Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report |volume=54 |issue=20 |pages=513–6 |pmid=15917736 |access-date=10 September 2017 |archive-date=5 November 2019 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20191105232114/https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5420a5.htm |url-status=live }}
<!--Gould 1981-->
*{{Cite journal |last=Moore |first=Elsie G |date=May 1986 |title=Family socialization and the IQ test performance of traditionally and transracially adopted Black children |journal=Developmental Psychology |volume=22 |issue=3 |pages=317–26 |doi=10.1037/0012-1649.22.3.317}}
*{{cite book |last=Gould |first=Stephen Jay |title=The Mismeasure of Man |url=https://archive.org/details/mismeasureofman00goulrich |url-access=registration |year=1981 |isbn=978-0-393-30056-7 |publisher=Norton |location=New York, London |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Moore |first1=David S. |last2=Shenk |first2=David |date=2016-12-01 |title=The heritability fallacy |journal=Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science |language=en |volume=8 |issue=1–2 |pages=e1400 |doi=10.1002/wcs.1400 |issn=1939-5078 |pmid=27906501}}
<!--Graves 2001-->
*{{Cite journal |last=Murray |first=Charles |year=2006 |title=Changes over time in the black–white difference on mental tests: Evidence from the children of the 1979 cohort of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth |journal=Intelligence |volume=34 |issue=6 |pages=527–540 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.004 |author-link=Charles Murray (political scientist)}}
*{{cite book |title=The Emperor's New Clothes: Biological Theories of Race at the Millennium |url=https://archive.org/details/emperorsnewcloth00grav |url-access=registration |last=Graves |first=Joseph L. |publisher=Rutgers University Press |year=2001 |asin=B000SARS70 |edition=Kindle |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last=Murray |first=Charles |date=July–August 2007 |title=The magnitude and components of change in the black–white IQ difference from 1920 to 1991: A birth cohort analysis of the Woodcock–Johnson standardizations |journal=Intelligence |volume=35 |issue=4 |pages=305–18 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2007.02.001}}
<!--Graves 2002a-->
*{{Cite journal |last=Neisser |first=Ulric |year=2004 |title=Serious Scientists or Disgusting Racists? |journal=Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of Books |volume=49 |issue=1 |pages=5–7 |doi=10.1037/004224 |author-link=Ulric Neisser}}
*{{Cite book |title=Race and Intelligence: Separating Myth from Reality |editor-first=Jefferson M |editor-last=Fish |isbn=978-0-8058-3757-5 |year=2002a |publisher=Laurence Erlbaum Associates |pages=57–94 |chapter=The Misuse of Life History Theory: JP Rushton and the Pseudoscience of Racial Hierarchy |last=Graves |first=Joseph L |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Neisser |first1=Ulric |last2=Boodoo |first2=Gwyneth |last3=Bouchard |first3=Thomas J Jr |last4=Boykin |first4=A. Wade |last5=Brody |first5=Nathan |last6=Ceci |first6=Stephen J |last7=Halpern |first7=Diane F |last8=Loehlin |first8=John C |last9=Perloff |first9=Robert |last10=Sternberg |first10=Robert J |last11=Urbina |first11=Susana |year=1996 |title=Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns |url=http://matt.colorado.edu/teaching/highcog/fall8/nbbbbchlpsu96.pdf |journal=American Psychologist |volume=51 |issue=2 |pages=77–101 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77 |access-date=2014-08-23 |archive-date=2015-04-20 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150420144859/http://matt.colorado.edu/teaching/highcog/fall8/nbbbbchlpsu96.pdf |url-status=live }}
<!--Graves 2002b -->
*{{Cite book |last=Nevid |first=Jeffrey S. |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=dO8bCgAAQBAJ |title=Essentials of Psychology: Concepts and Applications |date=2014-01-17 |publisher=Cengage Learning |isbn=978-1-285-75122-1 |language=en |access-date=2018-01-30 |archive-date=2023-03-20 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20230320043711/https://books.google.com/books?id=dO8bCgAAQBAJ |url-status=live }}
*{{cite journal | last=Graves | first=Joseph L, Jr | year=2002b | title=What a tangled web he weaves: Race, reproductive strategies and Rushton's life history theory | journal=Anthropological Theory | volume=2 | pages=131–54 | doi=10.1177/1469962002002002627 | issue=2 | ref=harv }}
*{{Cite journal |last=Nisbett |first=Richard |year=2005 |title=Heredity, environment, and race differences in IQ: A commentary on Rushton and Jensen |url=http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/30years/Nisbett-commentary-on-30years.pdf |journal=Psychology, Public Policy, and Law |volume=11 |issue=2 |pages=302–10 |doi=10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.302 |author-link=Richard E. Nisbett |access-date=2010-04-08 |archive-date=2011-06-05 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110605054810/http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/30years/Nisbett-commentary-on-30years.pdf |url-status=live }}
<!--Gray & Thompson 2004-->
*{{Cite book |last=Nisbett |first=Richard |url=https://archive.org/details/intelligencehowt00nisb |title=Intelligence and How to Get It: Why Schools and Cultures Count |publisher=W. W. Norton & Company |year=2009 |isbn=978-0-393-06505-3 }}
*{{cite journal |journal=Nature Reviews Neuroscience |volume=5 |pages=471–82 |year=2004 |title=Neurobiology of intelligence: science and ethics |first=Jeremy R |last=Gray |first2=Paul M |last2=Thompson |doi=10.1038/nrn1405 |pmid=15152197 |issue=6 |url=https://www.mun.ca/biology/scarr/Gray_&_Thompson_2004_Nature_Rev_Neurosci_5,471-482.pdf |ref=harv }}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |last3=Blair |first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens |first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |year=2012a |title=Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments |journal=American Psychologist |volume=67 |number=2 |pages=130–159 |issn=0003-066X |pmid=22233090 |doi=10.1037/a0026699}}
<!--Grossman & Kaufman 2001-->
**{{lay source |template=cite web |date=2012-05-10 |title=The latest on intelligence |url=http://www.danielwillingham.com/1/post/2012/05/the-latest-on-intelligence.html |website=Daniel Willingham}}
*{{Cite book |title=The evolution of intelligence |chapter=Evolutionary Psychology: Promise and Perils |last1=Grossman |first1=James B |last2=Kaufman |editor-first=Robert J |editor-last=Sternberg |editor2-first=James C |editor2-last=Kaufman |publisher=Routledge |year=2001 |isbn=978-0-8058-3267-9 |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |last3=Blair |first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens |first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |year=2012b |title=Group differences in IQ are best understood as environmental in origin |url=http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Nisbett%20(2012)%20Group.pdf |journal=American Psychologist |volume=67 |pages=503–504 |doi=10.1037/a0029772 |issn=0003-066X |pmid=22963427 |access-date=22 July 2013 |number=6 |archive-date=23 January 2015 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20150123114230/http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Nisbett%20(2012)%20Group.pdf |url-status=live }}
<!--Hampshire et al. 2012-->
*{{Cite book |last1=Niu |first1=Weihua |title=The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence |last2=Brass |first2=Jillian |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=2011 |isbn=978-0-521-51806-2 |editor-last=Sternberg |editor-first=Robert J |chapter=Intelligence in Worldwide Perspective |editor-last2=Kaufmann |editor-first2=Scott Barry}}
*{{Cite journal | last1 = Hampshire | first1 = A. | last2 = Highfield | first2 = R. R. | last3 = Parkin | first3 = B. L. | last4 = Owen | first4 = A. M. | year = 2012 | title = Fractionating human intelligence | journal = Neuron | volume = 76 | number = 6 | pages = 1225–1237 | ref = harv | doi=10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.022 | pmid=23259956}}
*{{Cite book |last=Ogbu |first=John |title=Minority Education and Caste: The American System in Cross-Cultural Perspective |publisher=Academic Press |year=1978 |isbn=978-0-125-24250-9 |location=New York}}
<!--Herrnstein & Murray 1994 -->
*{{Cite book |last=Ogbu |first=John U |title=Cross-cultural roots of minority child development |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |year=1994 |editor-last=Greenfield |editor-first=PM |location=Hillsdale, NJ |pages= |chapter=From cultural differences to differences in cultural frames of reference |editor-last2=Cocking |editor-first2=RR |chapter-url=https://archive.org/details/crossculturalroo00gree |chapter-url-access=registration }}
*{{cite book |first=Richard J |last=Herrnstein |first2=Charles |last2=Murray |year=1994 |title=The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life |url=https://archive.org/details/bellcurveintelli00herr |url-access=registration |publisher=Free Press |location=New York |isbn=978-0-02-914673-6 |ref=harv }}
*{{Cite journal |last=Olness |first=K |date=April 2003 |title=Effects on brain development leading to cognitive impairment: a worldwide epidemic |journal=Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics |volume=24 |issue=2 |pages=120–30 |doi=10.1097/00004703-200304000-00009 |pmid=12692458 |s2cid=31999992}}
<!--Ho et al. 1980-->
*{{Cite book |last1=Pickren |first1=Wade E |title=A History of Modern Psychology in Context |last2=Rutherford |first2=Alexandra |publisher=Wiley |year=2010 |author-link2=Alexandra Rutherford}}
*{{cite journal |title=Analysis of brain weight. I. Adult brain weight in relation to sex, race, and age. |first=KC |last=Ho |first2=U |last2=Roessmann |first3=JV |last3=Straumfjord |first4=G |last4=Monroe |journal=Archives of Pathology & Laboratory Medicine |volume=104 |issue=12 |date=December 1980 |pages=635–9 |ref={{harvid|Ho et al.|1980}} | pmid = 6893659}}
*{{Cite book |last1=Plotnik |first1=Rod |title=Introduction to Psychology |last2=Kouyoumdjian |first2=Haig |publisher=Cengage Learning |year=2011 |chapter=Intelligence}}<!--<-See "Binet's Two Warnings"-->
<!--Hunt 2010-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Qian |first1=M |last2=Wang |first2=D |last3=Watkins |first3=WE |last4=Gebski |first4=V |last5=Yan |first5=YQ |last6=Li |first6=M |last7=Chen |first7=ZP |year=2005 |title=The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China |journal=Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition |volume=14 |issue=1 |pages=32–42 |pmid=15734706 |ref={{harvid|Qian et al.|2005}}}}
*{{cite book |last=Hunt |first= Earl |title=Human Intelligence |publisher= Cambridge University Press |year=2010 |isbn=978-0-521-70781-7 |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last=Reichman |first=Nancy E. |date=2005 |title=Low birth weight and school readiness |journal=The Future of Children |volume=15 |issue=1 |pages=91–116 |doi=10.1353/foc.2005.0008 |issn=1054-8289 |pmid=16130543 |s2cid=23345980}}
<!--Hunt & Carlson 2007-->
*{{Cite encyclopedia |year=1988 |encyclopedia=Human Abilities in Cultural Context |publisher=Cambridge University Press |last=Reuning |first=Helmut |title=Testing Bushmen in the Central Kalahari |editor-last=Irvine |editor-first=SH |pages=453–486 |doi=10.1017/CBO9780511574603.018 |isbn=978-0-511-57460-3 |url=http://ebooks.cambridge.org/chapter.jsf?bid=CBO9780511574603&cid=CBO9780511574603A028 |editor2-last=Berry |editor2-first=JW |access-date=2010-04-26 |archive-date=2011-07-25 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110725133144/http://ebooks.cambridge.org/chapter.jsf?bid=CBO9780511574603&cid=CBO9780511574603A028 |url-status=live }}
*{{cite journal | last1 = Hunt |first1=Earl |last2= Carlson |first2=Jerry | year = 2007 | title = Considerations relating to the study of group differences in intelligence | journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science | volume = 2 | issue = 2| pages = 194–213 | doi = 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00037.x |pmid=26151960 |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last=Richardson |first=K |year=2004 |title=Book Review: IQ and the Wealth of Nations |journal=Heredity |volume=92 |issue=4 |pages=359–60 |doi=10.1038/sj.hdy.6800418 |doi-access=free}}
<!--Hunt & Wittmann 2008-->
*{{cite journal |first=Earl |last=Hunt |first2=Werner |last2=Wittmann |title=National intelligence and national prosperity |journal=Intelligence |volume=36 |date=January–February 2008 |pages=1–9 |issue=1 |doi= 10.1016/j.intell.2006.11.002 |ref={{harvid|Hunt|Wittmann|2008}} }} *{{Cite journal |last1=Rindermann |first1=Heiner |last2=Thompson |first2=James |date=November 2013 |title=Ability rise in NAEP and narrowing ethnic gaps? |journal=Intelligence |volume=41 |issue=6 |pages=821–831 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2013.06.016 |author-link1=Heiner Rindermann}}
*{{Cite journal |last=Rose |first=Steven |year=2009 |title=Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? NO: Science and society do not benefit |journal=Nature |volume=457 |issue=7231 |pages=786–8 |bibcode=2009Natur.457..786R |doi=10.1038/457786a |pmid=19212384 |s2cid=42846614}}
<!--Irvine 1983-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Roth |first1=PL |last2=Bevier |first2=CA |last3=Bobko |first3=P |last4=Switzer |first4=FS III |last5=Tyler |first5=P |year=2001 |title=Ethnic group differences in cognitive ability in employment and educational settings: A metaanalysis |journal=Personnel Psychology |volume=54 |issue=2 |pages=297–330 |citeseerx=10.1.1.372.6092 |doi=10.1111/j.1744-6570.2001.tb00094.x |ref={{harvid|Roth et al.|2001}}}}
*{{cite journal |journal=Nature |volume=302 |page=371 |year=1983 |doi=10.1038/302371b0 |first=SH |last=Irvine |title=Where intelligence tests fail |ref= harv |issue=5907|bibcode = 1983Natur.302..371I }}
*{{Cite book |last=Rouvroy |first=Antoinette |title=Human genes and neoliberal governance: a Foucauldian critique |publisher=Routledge |year=2008 |isbn=978-0-415-44433-0 |page=86}}
<!--Irvine & Berry 1988-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Rowe |first1=David C |date=January 2005 |editor-last=Rodgers |editor-first=Joseph |title=Under the Skin: On the Impartial Treatment of Genetic and Environmental Hypothesis of Racial Differences |journal=American Psychologist |volume=60 |issue=1 |pages=60–70 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.60 |pmid=15641922}}
*{{Cite book |isbn=978-0-521-34482-1 |title=Human Abilities in Cultural Context |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=1988 |editor-first=SH |editor-last=Irvine |editor2-first=JW |editor2-last=Berry |ref={{harvid|Irvine|Berry|1988}} |url=https://archive.org/details/humanabilitiesin0000unse }}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Rushton |first1=J. Philippe |last2=Jensen |first2=Arthur R |year=2005 |title=Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability |url=http://psychology.uwo.ca/faculty/rushtonpdfs/PPPL1.pdf |journal=Psychology, Public Policy, and Law |volume=11 |issue=2 |pages=246–8 |citeseerx=10.1.1.186.102 |doi=10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20151103215722/http://psychology.uwo.ca/faculty/rushtonpdfs/PPPL1.pdf |archive-date=2015-11-03}}
<!--Ivanovic et al. 2004-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Saloojee |first1=Haroon |last2=Pettifor |first2=John M |date=December 2001 |title=Iron deficiency and impaired child development |journal=BMJ |volume=323 |issue=7326 |pages=1377–8 |doi=10.1136/bmj.323.7326.1377 |pmc=1121846 |pmid=11744547}}
*{{cite journal |first1=Daniza M. |last1=Ivanovic |first2=Boris P. |last2=Leiva |first3=Hernán T. |last3=Pérez |first4=Manuel G. |last4=Olivares |first5=Nora S. |last5=Dı́az |first6=Marı́a Soledad C. |last6=Urruti |first7=Atilio F. |last7=Almagià |first8=Triana D. |last8=Toro |first9=Patricio T. |last9=Miller |first10=Enrique O. |last10=Bosch |first11=Cristián G. |last11=Larraı́n |title=Head size and intelligence, learning, nutritional status and brain development. Head, IQ, learning, nutrition and brain |journal=Neuropsychologia |volume=42 |issue=8 |pages=1118–31 |year=2004 |pmid=15093150 |doi= 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.022 |ref={{harvid|Ivanovic et al.|2004}} }}
*{{Cite book |last1=Scarr |first1=S. |title=Methods of family research: Biographies of research projects |last2=Weinberg |first2=R. A. |year=1990 |volume=1 |pages=121–151 |chapter=The nature-nurture problem revisited: The Minnesota adoption studies}}
<!--Jackson & Weidman 2004-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Scarr |first1=S. |last2=Weinberg |first2=R. A. |year=1976 |title=IQ test performance of black children adopted by White families |journal=American Psychologist |volume=31 |issue=10 |pages=726–739 |doi=10.1037/0003-066x.31.10.726 |s2cid=32410011 }}
*{{cite book|title=Race, Racism, and Science: Social Impact and Interaction|year=2004|first=John P. |last=Jackson|first2=Nadine M. |last2=Weidman|publisher=ABC-CLIO|page=23|ref=harv}}
*{{Cite book |last1=Schacter |first1=Daniel |title=Psychology |last2=Gilbert |first2=Daniel |last3=Wegner |first3=Daniel |publisher=Worth Publishing |year=2007 |isbn=978-0-7167-5215-8 |location=New York |url-access=registration |url=https://archive.org/details/psychology0000scha_e2x8 }}
<!--Jackson 2005-->
*{{Cite book |last=James A. Banks |title=Encyclopedia of Race, Ethnicity and Society |publisher=SAGE |year=2008 |editor-last=Schaefer |editor-first=Richard T. |volume=2 |page=1091 |chapter=Race |ref={{harvid|Schaefer|2008}} |author-link=James A. Banks}}
*{{Cite book |title=Science for Segregation: Race, Law, and the Case against Brown v. Board of Education |last=Jackson |first= John P. |publisher=] |year=2005 |isbn=978-0-8147-4271-6 |layurl=http://www.historycooperative.org/journals/lhr/25.2/br_19.html |laydate=30 August 2010|ref=harv}}
*{{Cite book |last=Serpell |first=Robert |title=Handbook of Intelligence |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=2000 |isbn=978-0-521-59648-0 |editor-last=Sternberg |editor-first=Robert J. |pages=549–577 |chapter=Intelligence and Culture}}
<!--Jensen 1969-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Sheppard |first1=Leah D. |last2=Vernon |first2=Philip A. |date=February 2008 |title=Intelligence and speed of information-processing: A review of 50 years of research |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |volume=44 |issue=3 |pages=535–551 |doi=10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.015}}
*{{cite journal |last=Jensen |first=Arthur R |year=1969 |title=How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement? |journal=Harvard Educational Review |volume=39 |pages=1–123 |ref=harv|title-link=How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement? |doi=10.17763/haer.39.1.l3u15956627424k7 }}
*{{Cite book |last=Shurkin |first=Joel |title=Broken Genius: The Rise and Fall of William Shockley, Creator of the Electronic Age |publisher=Macmillan |year=2006 |isbn=978-1-4039-8815-7 |location=London}}
<!--Jensen 1973-->
**{{lay source |template=cite magazine |author=Brian Clegg |title=Review - Broken Genius - Joel Shurkin |url=http://www.popularscience.co.uk/reviews/rev291.htm |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20061006151557/http://www.popularscience.co.uk/reviews/rev291.htm |archive-date=2006-10-06 |magazine=Popular Science}}
*{{cite book |last=Jensen |first=Arthur R |year=1973 |title=Educability and Group Differences |location=London |publisher=Methuen |isbn=978-0-06-012194-5 |ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Shuttleworth-Edwards |first1=AB |last2=Kemp |first2=RD |last3=Rust |first3=AL |last4=Muirhead |first4=JG |last5=Hartman |first5=NP |last6=Radloff |first6=SE |date=October 2004 |title=Cross-cultural effects on IQ test performance: a review and preliminary normative indications on WAIS-III test performance |journal=Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology |volume=26 |issue=7 |pages=903–20 |doi=10.1080/13803390490510824 |pmid=15742541 |s2cid=16060622 |ref={{harvid|Shuttleworth-Edwards et al.|2004}}}}
<!--Jensen 1993-->
*{{cite journal | last1 = Jensen |first1=Arthur R |last2=Whang |first2=PA | year = 1993 | title = Reaction times and intelligence: a comparison of Chinese-American and Anglo-American children | url = | journal = Journal of Biosocial Science | volume = 25 | issue = 3| pages = 397–410 | pmid = 8360233 |ref=harv | doi=10.1017/s0021932000020721}} *{{Cite journal |last1=Smay |first1=Diana |last2=Armelagos |first2=George |date=July 2000 |title=Galileo Wept: A Critical Assessment of the Use of Race in Forensic Anthropology |journal=Transforming Anthropology |volume=9 |issue=2 |pages=19–29 |doi=10.1525/tran.2000.9.2.19|s2cid=143942539 }}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Snyderman |first1=Mark |last2=Rothman |first2=Stanley |year=1987 |title=Survey of expert opinion on intelligence and aptitude testing |journal=American Psychologist |volume=42 |issue=2 |pages=137–44 |doi=10.1037/0003-066x.42.2.137}}
<!--Jensen 1998 -->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Sternberg |first1=Robert J |last2=Grigorenko |first2=Elena L |last3=Kidd |first3=Kenneth K |year=2005 |title=Intelligence, Race, and Genetics |url=http://medicine.yale.edu/labs/kidd/440.pdf |journal=American Psychologist |volume=60 |issue=1 |pages=46–59 |citeseerx=10.1.1.174.313 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.46 |pmid=15641921 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110720180251/http://medicine.yale.edu/labs/kidd/440.pdf |archive-date=2011-07-20 }}
*{{cite book |last=Jensen |first=Arthur R |year=1998 |title=The g factor: The science of mental ability |location=Westport, CT |publisher=Praeger |isbn=978-0-275-96103-9 |ref=harv |url=https://archive.org/details/gfactorscienc00jens }}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Sternberg |first1=R. J. |last2=Grigorenko |first2=E. L. |year=2007 |title=The Difficulty of Escaping Preconceptions in Writing an Article About the Difficulty of Escaping Preconceptions: Commentary on Hunt and Carlson (2007) |journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science |volume=2 |issue=2 |pages=221–223 |doi=10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00040.x |pmid=26151963 |s2cid=21096328}}
<!--Jensen 2006-->
*{{cite book |last=Jensen |first=AR |year=2006 |title=Clocking the mind: Mental chronometry and individual differences |location=Amsterdam |publisher=Elsevier |isbn=978-0-08-044939-5 |ref=harv }} *{{Cite book |last=Spiro |first=Jonathan P. |title=Defending the Master Race: Conservation, Eugenics, and the Legacy of Madison Grant |publisher=Univ. of Vermont Press |year=2009 |isbn=978-1-58465-715-6}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Suzuki |first1=Lisa |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |date=2005 |title=The cultural malleability of intelligence and its impact on the racial/ethnic hierarchy. |journal=Psychology, Public Policy, and Law |volume=11 |issue=2 |pages=320–327 |citeseerx=10.1.1.1022.3693 |doi=10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.320}}
<!--Jensen & Johnson 1994 -->
*<!--to identify citation in quotation in mainspace from Hunt & Carlson (2007)-->{{Cite journal |last1=Tang |first1=Hua |last2=Quertermous |first2=Tom |last3=Rodriguez |first3=Beatriz |last4=Kardia |first4=Sharon L.R. |last5=Zhu |first5=Xiaofeng |last6=Brown |first6=Andrew |last7=Pankow |first7=James S. |last8=Province |first8=Michael A. |last9=Hunt |first9=Steven C. |last10=Boerwinkle |first10=Eric |last11=Schork |first11=Nicholas J. |date=February 2005 |title=Genetic structure, self-identified race/ethnicity, and confounding in case-control association studies |journal=American Journal of Human Genetics |volume=76 |issue=2 |pages=268–75 |doi=10.1086/427888 |pmc=1196372 |pmid=15625622 |ref={{harvid|Tang et al.|2005}} |first12=Neil J. |last12=Risch}}
*{{Cite journal |doi=10.1016/0160-2896(94)90032-9 |last2=Johnson |first2=Fred W |last1=Jensen |first1=Arthur R |date=May–June 1994 |title=Race and sex differences in head size and IQ |journal=Intelligence |volume=18 |pages=309–33 |issue=3 |ref=harv }}
*{{Citation |last=Templeton |first=A. R. |title=Race and Intelligence Separating Science from Myth |date=2001 |pages=31–55 |editor-last=Fish |editor-first=J. M. |contribution=The Genetic and Evolutionary Significance of Human Races |place=London |publisher=Routledge |isbn=978-0-8058-3757-5 |author-link=Alan R. Templeton}}
<!--Kamin 2006-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Thompson |first1=Paul M. |last2=Cannon |first2=Tyrone D. |last3=Narr |first3=Katherine L. |last4=van Erp |first4=Theo |last5=Poutanen |first5=Veli-Pekka |last6=Huttunen |first6=Matti |last7=Lönnqvist |first7=Jouko |last8=Standertskjöld-Nordenstam |first8=Carl-Gustaf |last9=Kaprio |first9=Jaakko |last10=Khaledy |first10=Mohammad |last11=Dail |first11=Rajneesh |year=2001 |title=Genetic influences on brain structure |url=http://www.loni.ucla.edu/~thompson/MEDIA/NN/Nature_Neuro2001_genetics.pdf |journal=Nature Neuroscience |volume=4 |issue=12 |pages=1253–58 |doi=10.1038/nn758 |pmid=11694885 |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20110515085921/http://www.loni.ucla.edu/~thompson/MEDIA/NN/Nature_Neuro2001_genetics.pdf |archive-date=2011-05-15 |ref={{harvid|Thompson et al.|2001}} |first12=Chris I. |last12=Zoumalan |first13=Arthur W. |last13=Toga |s2cid=7132470 }}
*{{cite journal|last1=Kamin|first1=Leon J.|title=African IQ and Mental Retardation|journal=South African Journal of Psychology|date=March 2006|volume=36|issue=1|pages=1–9|doi=10.1177/008124630603600101|ref=harv}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Tizard |first1=Barbara |last2=Cooperman |first2=Oliver |last3=Joseph |first3=Anne |last4=Tizard |first4=Jack |date=June 1972 |title=Environmental effects on language development: A study of young children in long-stay residential nurseries |journal=Child Development |volume=43 |issue=2 |pages=337–58 |doi=10.2307/1127540 |jstor=1127540 |ref={{harvid|Tizard et al.|1972}}}}
<!--Kaplan 2011-->
*{{Cite book |last=Tucker |first=William H |title=The Funding of Scientific Racism: Wickliffe Draper and the Pioneer Fund |publisher=University of Illinois Press |year=2002 |isbn=978-0-252-02762-8 |author-link=William H. Tucker (psychologist)}}
*{{cite book |last=Kaplan |first=Jonathan Michael |date=January 2011 |title='Race': What Biology Can Tell Us about a Social Construct |journal=Encyclopedia of Life Sciences |url=http://www.els.net/WileyCDA/ElsArticle/refId-a0005857.html |doi=10.1002/9780470015902.a0005857 |ref={{harvid|Kaplan|2011}} |isbn=978-0470016176 }}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Verney |first1=Steven P. |last2=Granholm |first2=Eric |last3=Marshall |first3=Sandra P. |last4=Malcarne |first4=Vanessa L. |last5=Saccuzzo |first5=Dennis P. |date=26 July 2016 |title=Culture-Fair Cognitive Ability Assessment |journal=Assessment |volume=12 |issue=3 |pages=303–319 |doi=10.1177/1073191105276674 |pmid=16123251 |s2cid=31024437 |ref={{harvid|Verney et al.|2005}}}}
<!--Kaplan 2015-->
*{{cite journal|last1=Kaplan|first1=Jonathan Michael|title=Race, IQ, and the search for statistical signals associated with so-called "X"-factors: environments, racism, and the "hereditarian hypothesis"|journal=Biology & Philosophy|date=6 February 2014|volume=30|issue=1|pages=1–17|doi=10.1007/s10539-014-9428-0|ref={{harvid|Kaplan|2015}}}} *{{Cite journal |last=Vincent |first=Ken R. |date=March 1991 |title=Black/white IQ differences: Does age make the difference? |journal=Journal of Clinical Psychology |volume=47 |issue=2 |pages=266–270 |doi=10.1002/1097-4679(199103)47:2<266::aid-jclp2270470213>3.0.co;2-s |pmid=2030133}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Weinberg |first1=Richard A. |last2=Scarr |first2=Sandra |last3=Waldman |first3=Irwin D. |year=1992 |title=The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study: A Follow-Up of IQ Test Performance at Adolescence |journal=Intelligence |volume=16 |issue=1 |pages=117–35 |doi=10.1016/0160-2896(92)90028-P}}
<!--Kaszycka, Štrkalj & Strzałko 2009-->
*{{cite journal |first1=Katarzyna A. |last1=Kaszycka |first2=Goran |last2=Štrkalj |first3=Jan |last3=Strzałko |title=Current Views of European Anthropologists on Race: Influence of Educational and Ideological Background |year=2009 |doi=10.1111/j.1548-1433.2009.01076.x |journal=American Anthropologist |volume=111 |issue=1 |pages=43–56 |ref=harv }} *{{Cite journal |last1=Wicherts |first1=Jelte M |last2=Borsboom |first2=Denny |last3=Dolan |first3=Conor V |year=2010 |title=Why national IQs do not support evolutionary theories of intelligence |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |volume=48 |issue=2 |pages=91–6 |doi=10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.028}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Wicherts |first1=Jelte M. |last2=Borsboom |first2=Denny |last3=Dolan |first3=Conor V. |year=2010b |title=Evolution, brain size, and the national IQ of peoples around 3000 years B.C |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |volume=48 |issue=2 |pages=104–106 |doi=10.1016/j.paid.2009.08.020}}
<!--Lieberman 2001 -->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Wicherts |first1=Jelte M. |last2=Dolan |first2=Conor V. |last3=Carlson |first3=Jerry S. |last4=van der Maas |first4=Han L.J. |date=June 2010 |title=Raven's test performance of sub-Saharan Africans: Average performance, psychometric properties, and the Flynn Effect |journal=Learning and Individual Differences |volume=20 |issue=3 |pages=135–151 |doi=10.1016/j.lindif.2009.12.001}}
*{{cite journal|last=Lieberman|first=Leonard|title=How "Caucasoids" got such big crania and why they shrank: from Morton to Rushton."|journal=Current Anthropology |volume=42|issue=1|year=2001|pages=69–95|ref=harv|doi=10.1086/318434|pmid=14992214}}
*{{Cite journal |last1=Wicherts |first1=Jelte M. |last2=Dolan |first2=Conor V. |last3=van der Maas |first3=Han L.J. |date=January 2010 |title=A systematic literature review of the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans |journal=Intelligence |volume=38 |issue=1 |pages=1–20 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.002}}
<!--Loehlin 2000 -->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Witelson |first1=S. F. |last2=Beresh |first2=H. |last3=Kigar |first3=D. L. |date=February 2006 |title=Intelligence and brain size in 100 postmortem brains: sex, lateralization and age factors |journal=Brain |volume=129 |issue=2 |pages=386–398 |doi=10.1093/brain/awh696 |pmid=16339797 |doi-access=free}}
*{{cite book |last=Loehlin |first=John C |year=2000 |chapter=Group Differences in Intelligence |editor-first=Robert J |editor-last= Sternberg |title=The Handbook of Intelligence |location=Cambridge |publisher=Cambridge University Press |ref=harv }}
*{{Cite book |last=Wooldridge |first=Adrian |title=Measuring the Mind: Education and Psychology in England c. 1860-c. 1990 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=1995 |isbn=978-0-521-39515-1}}
<!--Ludy 2006-->
*{{Cite book |title=Brief History of Modern Psychology |last=Ludy |first=Benjamin T|publisher=Wiley-Blackwell |year=2006 |isbn=978-1-4051-3206-0 |pages=188–91 |ref=harv }} *{{Cite book |last=Wroe |first=Andrew |title=The Republican Party and Immigration Politics: from Proposition 187 to George W. Bush |publisher=Macmillan |year=2008 |isbn=978-0-230-60053-9 |page=294}}
<!--Lynn Vanhanen 2002 -->
*{{cite book |last=Lynn |first=Richard |last2= Vanhanen |first2=Tatu |year= 2002 |title=IQ and the wealth of nations |location=Westport, CT |publisher=Praeger |isbn=978-0-275-97510-4 |ref=harv }}
<!--MacEachern 2006 -->
*{{cite journal|last1=MacEachern|first1=Scott|title=Africanist archaeology and ancient IQ: racial science and cultural evolution in the twenty-first century|journal=World Archaeology|date=March 2006|volume=38|issue=1|pages=72–92|doi=10.1080/00438240500509918|ref=harv}}
<!--Mackenzie 1984-->
*{{Cite journal |last=Mackenzie |first=Brian |date=1984 |title=Explaining race differences in IQ: The logic, the methodology, and the evidence. |journal=American Psychologist |language=en |volume=39 |issue=11 |pages=1214–1233 |doi=10.1037/0003-066x.39.11.1214|ref=harv}}
<!--Mackintosh 1998 -->
*{{Cite book |first=NJ |last=Mackintosh |title=IQ and Human Intelligence |year=1998 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0-19-852367-3 |lay-url=http://www.oup.com/us/catalog/general/subject/Psychology/CognitivePsychology/?view=usa&ci=9780198523673 |laydate=9 August 2010 |ref=harv |url=https://archive.org/details/iqhumanintellige00mack }}
<!--Mackintosh 2011 -->
*{{Cite book |title=IQ and Human Intelligence |last=Mackintosh |first=N. J. |authorlink=Nicholas Mackintosh |year=2011 |edition=second |publisher=Oxford University Press |location=Oxford |isbn=978-0-19-958559-5|lay-url=http://www.oupcanada.com/catalog/9780199585595.html |laydate=9 February 2012|ref=harv}}
<!--Manly et al. 2004-->
*{{cite journal |title=Acculturation, Reading Level, and Neuropsychological Test Performance Among African American Elders |first=JJ |last=Manly |first2=DA |last2=Byrd |first3=P |last3=Touradji |first4=Yaakov |last4=Stern |year=2004 |journal=Applied Neuropsychology |pmid=15471745 |issue=1 |pages=37–46|ref={{harvid|Manly et al.|2004}} |doi=10.1207/s15324826an1101_5 |volume=11}}
<!--Manly et al. 2002 -->
*{{cite journal |title=Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance between African American and White elders |first=Jennifer J |last=Manly |first2=Diane M |last2=Jacobs |first3=Pegah |last3=Touradji |first4=Scott A |last4=Small |first5=Yaakov |last5=Stern |journal=Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society |doi=10.1017/S1355617702813157 |pmid=11939693 |year=2002 |ref={{harvid|Manly et al.|2002}} |volume=8 |issue=3 |pages=341–348}}
<!--Maltby, Day & Macaskill 2007-->
*{{Cite book |first1=John |last1=Maltby |first2=Liz |last2=Day |first3=Ann |last3=Macaskill |publisher=] |year=2007 |title=Personality, Individual Differences and Intelligence |isbn= 978-0-13-129760-9 |pages=334–47 |ref=harv }}
<!--McDaniel 2005-->
*{{cite journal | last1 = McDaniel | first1 = M. A. | year = 2005 | title = Big-brained people are smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship between in vivo brain volume and intelligence | url = | journal = Intelligence | volume = 33 | issue = 4| pages = 337–346 | doi=10.1016/j.intell.2004.11.005 | ref={{harvid|McDaniel|2005}} }}
<!--Mickelson 2003 -->
*<!--note not published? not peer reviewed->-->{{cite journal |title=When Are Racial Disparities in Education the Result of Racial Discrimination? A Social Science Perspective |first= Roslyn Arlin |last=Mickelson |year=2003 |ref=harv }}
<!--Miele 2002-->
*{{cite book |first=Frank |last=Miele |title=Intelligence, Race and Genetics: Conversations with Arthur R. Jensen |publisher= Basic Books |year= 2002 |page=256 |ref=harv }}
<!--MMWR 2005 -->
*{{cite journal |last=MMWR |url=https://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/preview/mmwrhtml/mm5420a5.htm |title=Blood Lead Levels—United States, 1999–2002 |journal=Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report (MMWR)|date=27 May 2005 |issue=20 |volume=54 |pages=513–6 |ref={{harvid|MMWR|2005}}}}
<!--Moore 1986 -->
*{{cite journal |first=Elsie G |last=Moore |title=Family socialization and the IQ test performance of traditionally and transracially adopted Black children |journal=Developmental Psychology |volume= 22 |pages=317–26 |date=May 1986 |url=http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/dev/22/3/317/ |doi=10.1037/0012-1649.22.3.317 |issue=3 |ref={{harvid|Moore|1986}} }}
<!--Moore & Shenk 2016-->
*{{Cite journal |last=Moore |first=David S. |last2=Shenk |first2=David |date=2016-12-01 |title=The heritability fallacy |journal=Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science |language=en |volume=8 |issue=1–2 |pages=e1400 |doi=10.1002/wcs.1400 |pmid=27906501 |issn=1939-5078|ref={{harvid|Moore|Shenk|2016}}}}
<!--Murray 2006 -->
*{{cite journal |title=Changes over time in the black–white difference on mental tests: Evidence from the children of the 1979 cohort of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth |first=Charles |last=Murray |journal=Intelligence |year=2006 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.004 |volume=34 |issue=6 |pages=527–540 |ref=harv }}
<!--Murray 2007-->
*{{cite journal |title=The magnitude and components of change in the black–white IQ difference from 1920 to 1991: A birth cohort analysis of the Woodcock–Johnson standardizations |first=Charles |last=Murray |journal=Intelligence |volume=35 |issue=4 |date= July–August 2007 |pages=305–18 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2007.02.001 |ref={{harvid|Murray|2007}}}}
<!--Neisser 2004-->
*{{cite journal |last=Neisser |first=Ulric |year=2004 |title=Serious Scientists or Disgusting Racists? |journal=Contemporary Psychology: APA Review of Books |volume=49 |issue=1 |pages=5–7 |doi=10.1037/004224 |ref=harv}}
<!--Neisser et al. 1996-->
*{{cite journal |doi= 10.1037/0003-066X.51.2.77 |last1=Neisser |first1=Ulric |last2=Boodoo|first2=Gwyneth |last3= Bouchard |first3=Thomas J, Jr |last4=Boykin |first4=A. Wade |last5=Brody |first5=Nathan |last6=Ceci |first6=Stephen J |last7=Halpern |first7=Diane F |last8=Loehlin |first8=John C |last9=Perloff |first9=Robert |last10=Sternberg |first10=Robert J |last11=Urbina |first11=Susana |title=Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns |journal=American Psychologist |volume=51 |issue=2 |pages=77–101 |year=1996 |url=http://matt.colorado.edu/teaching/highcog/fall8/nbbbbchlpsu96.pdf |ref=harv }}
<!--Nevid 2014-->
*{{Cite book |url=https://books.google.com/books?id=dO8bCgAAQBAJ |title=Essentials of Psychology: Concepts and Applications |last=Nevid |first=Jeffrey S. |date=2014-01-17 |publisher=Cengage Learning |isbn=9781285751221 |language=en|ref=harv}}
<!--Nisbett 2005-->
*{{cite journal |first=Richard |last=Nisbett |title=Heredity, environment, and race differences in IQ: A commentary on Rushton and Jensen |journal=Psychology, Public Policy, and Law |volume=11 |pages=302–10 |year=2005 |url=http://www.udel.edu/educ/gottfredson/30years/Nisbett-commentary-on-30years.pdf |doi=10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.302 |issue=2 |ref=harv}}
<!--Nisbett 2009-->
*{{cite book |last=Nisbett |first=Richard |title=Intelligence and How to Get It: Why Schools and Cultures Count |publisher=W. W. Norton & Company |year=2009 |isbn=978-0-393-06505-3 |ref=harv |url=https://archive.org/details/intelligencehowt00nisb }}
<!-- Nisbett et al. 2012-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Nisbett |first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |last3=Blair |first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens |first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |title=Intelligence: new findings and theoretical developments |journal=American Psychologist |doi=10.1037/a0026699 |pmid=22233090 |issn=0003-066X |volume=67 |number=2 |year=2012 |pages=130–159 |url=http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/nisbett2012int.pdf |accessdate=22 July 2013 |lay-url=http://www.danielwillingham.com/1/post/2012/05/the-latest-on-intelligence.html |laydate=22 July 2013 |ref=harv }}
<!-- Nisbett et al. 2012b-->
*{{Cite journal |last1=Nisbett|first1=Richard E. |last2=Aronson|first2=Joshua |last3=Blair|first3=Clancy |last4=Dickens|first4=William |last5=Flynn |first5=James |last6=Halpern |first6=Diane F. |last7=Turkheimer |first7=Eric |title=Group differences in IQ are best understood as environmental in origin |journal=American Psychologist |doi=10.1037/a0029772 |pmid=22963427 |issn=0003-066X |volume=67 |number=6 |year=2012b |pages=503–504 |url=http://people.virginia.edu/~ent3c/papers2/Articles%20for%20Online%20CV/Nisbett%20(2012)%20Group.pdf |accessdate=22 July 2013 |lay-url=http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/amp/67/6/503 |laydate=22 July 2013 |ref=harv}}
<!--Niu & Brass 2011-->
*{{cite book |title=The Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence |last1=Niu |first1=Weihua |last2=Brass |first2=Jillian |editor1-last=Sternberg |editor1-first=Robert J |editor2-last=Kaufmann |editor2-first=Scott Barry |chapter=Intelligence in Worldwide Perspective |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=2011 |isbn=978-0-521-51806-2 |ref=harv}}
<!--Ogbu 1978 -->
*{{cite journal |last=Ogbu |first=John |year= 1978 |title=Minority education and caste: The American system in cross-cultural perspective |location=New York |publisher= Academic Press |ref=harv }}
<!--Ogbu 1994 -->
*{{cite book |last=Ogbu |first=John U |year= 1994 |chapter=From cultural differences to differences in cultural frames of reference|editor1-first=PM |editor1-last= Greenfield |editor2-first=RR |editor2-last= Cocking |title=Cross-cultural roots of minority child development |chapter-url=https://archive.org/details/crossculturalroo00gree |chapter-url-access=registration |pages= |location= Hillsdale, NJ |publisher=Lawrence Erlbaum Associates |ref=harv }}
<!--Olness 2003 -->
*{{cite journal |last=Olness |first=K |title=Effects on brain development leading to cognitive impairment: a worldwide epidemic |journal=Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics |volume=24 |issue=2 |pages=120–30 |date=April 2003 |pmid=12692458 |doi=10.1097/00004703-200304000-00009 |ref=harv}}
<!--Onwuegbozie & Daley 2001-->
*{{cite journal|last1=Onwuegbuzie|first1=A. J.|last2=Daley|first2=C. E.|title=Racial Differences in IQ Revisited: A Synthesis of Nearly a Century of Research|journal=Journal of Black Psychology|date=1 May 2001|volume=27|issue=2|pages=209–220|doi=10.1177/0095798401027002004|ref=harv}}
<!--Pesta & Poznanski 2008-->
*{{cite journal |last=Pesta |first=Bryan J |last2=Poznanski |first2=Peter J |year= 2008 |title=Black-White differences on IQ and grades: The mediating role of elementary cognitive tasks |journal=Intelligence |volume=36 |issue=4 |pages=323–29 |ref=harv |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2007.07.004|url=http://engagedscholarship.csuohio.edu/bus_facpub/10 }}
<!--Pesta & Poznanski 2014-->
*{{Cite journal|last=Pesta|first=Bryan J|last2=Poznanski|first2=Peter J|year=2014|title=Only in America: Cold Winters Theory, race, IQ and well-being|journal=Intelligence|volume=46|pages=271–274|doi=10.1016/j.intell.2014.07.009|ref=harv|url=https://engagedscholarship.csuohio.edu/bus_facpub/98}}
<!--Pickren 2010-->
*{{cite book |title=A History of Modern Psychology in Context |first1=Wade E |last1=Pickren |first2=Alexandra |last2=Rutherford |publisher=Wiley |year=2010 |ref=harv}}
<!--Plomin, Kennedy & Craig 2005 -->
*{{cite journal |title=The quest for quantitative trait loci associated with intelligence |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2006.01.001 |last=Plomin |first=R |last2=Kennedy |first2=J |last3=Craig |first3=I |year=2005 |journal=Intelligence |volume=34 |pages=513–526 |ref=harv |issue=6 }}
<!--Plotnik & Kouyoumdjian 2010-->
*{{cite book |first1=Rod |last1=Plotnik |first2=Haig |last2=Kouyoumdjian |title=Introduction to Psychology |publisher= Cengage Learning |year=2011 |chapter=Intelligence |ref=harv}}<!--<-See "Binet's Two Warnings"-->
<!--Posthuma et al. 2002 -->
*{{cite journal |first=Daniëlle |last=Posthuma |first2=Eco JC |last2=De Geus |first3=Wim FC |last3=Baaré |first4=Hilleke E |last4=Hulshoff Pol |first5=René S |last5=Kahn |first6=Dorret I |last6= Boomsma |date=February 2002 |title=The association between brain volume and intelligence is of genetic origin |journal=Nature Neuroscience |volume=5 |pages=83–4 |doi= 10.1038/nn0202-83 |pmid=11818967 |issue=2 |ref={{harvid|Posthuma et al.|2002}}|hdl=1871/18295 |url=https://research.vu.nl/ws/files/1823021/Posthuma%20Nature%20Neuroscience%205(2)%202002%20u.pdf }}
<!--Qian et al. 2005-->
*{{cite journal |last1=Qian |first1=M |last2=Wang |first2=D |last3=Watkins |first3=WE |last4=Gebski |first4=V |last5=Yan |first5=YQ |last6=Li |first6=M |last7=Chen |first7=ZP |title=The effects of iodine on intelligence in children: a meta-analysis of studies conducted in China |journal=Asia Pacific Journal of Clinical Nutrition |volume=14 |issue=1 |pages=32–42 |year=2005 |pmid=15734706 |ref={{harvid|Qian et al.|2005}} }}
<!--Reichman 2005-->
*{{Cite journal |last=Reichman |first=Nancy E. |date=2005 |title=Low birth weight and school readiness |journal=The Future of Children |volume=15 |issue=1 |pages=91–116 |issn=1054-8289 |pmid=16130543|ref={{harvid|Reichman|2005}}|doi=10.1353/foc.2005.0008 }}
<!--Reuning 1988-->
*{{cite encyclopedia |chapter-url=http://ebooks.cambridge.org/chapter.jsf?bid=CBO9780511574603&cid=CBO9780511574603A028 |last=Reuning |first=Helmut |encyclopedia=Human Abilities in Cultural Context |editor1-last=Irvine |editor1-first=SH |editor2-last=Berry |editor2-first=JW |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=1988 |doi=10.1017/CBO9780511574603.018 |isbn=978-0-511-57460-3 |ref=harv|pages=453–486 |chapter=Testing Bushmen in the Central Kalahari }}
<!--Richardson 2004-->
*{{cite journal |first=K |last=Richardson |title=Book Review: IQ and the Wealth of Nations |journal=Heredity |volume=92 |pages=359–60 |year=2004 |doi=10.1038/sj.hdy.6800418 |issue=4 |ref=harv}}
<!--Rindermann & Thompson 2013-->
*{{cite journal|last1=Rindermann|first1=Heiner|last2=Thompson|first2=James|title=Ability rise in NAEP and narrowing ethnic gaps?|journal=Intelligence|date=November 2013|volume=41|issue=6|pages=821–831|doi=10.1016/j.intell.2013.06.016|ref=harv}}
<!--Rose 2009-->
*{{cite journal |first=Steven |last=Rose |year=2009 |title=Darwin 200: Should scientists study race and IQ? NO: Science and society do not benefit |journal=Nature |volume=457 |pages=786–8 |doi=10.1038/457786a |pmid=19212384 |issue=7231 |bibcode=2009Natur.457..786R |ref=harv}}
<!--Roth et al. 2001 -->
*{{cite journal |last1=Roth |first1=PL |last2=Bevier |first2= CA |last3=Bobko |first3=P |last4=Switzer |first4=FS, III |last5=Tyler |first5=P |year=2001 |title=Ethnic group differences in cognitive ability in employment and educational settings: A metaanalysis |journal=Personnel Psychology |volume=54 |issue=2 |pages= 297–330 |ref={{harvid|Roth et al.|2001}} |doi=10.1111/j.1744-6570.2001.tb00094.x|citeseerx=10.1.1.372.6092 }}
<!--Rouvroy 2008-->
*{{Cite book |title=Human genes and neoliberal governance: a Foucauldian critique |first=Antoinette |last=Rouvroy |page=86 |year=2008 |isbn=978-0-415-44433-0 |ref=harv |publisher=Routledge }}
<!--Rowe 2005 -->
*{{cite journal |last1=Rowe |last2=David C |editor-last=Rodgers |editor-first=Joseph |title=Under the Skin: On the Impartial Treatment of Genetic and Environmental Hypothesis of Racial Differences |journal=American Psychologist |volume=60 |issue=1 |date=January 2005 |pages=60–70 |url=http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=buy.optionToBuy&id=2005-00117-007 |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.60 |ref={{harvid|Rowe|2005}} |pmid=15641922}}
<!--Rushton 1995-->
*{{cite book |last=Rushton |first=J. Philippe |year=1995 |title=Race, evolution, and Behavior |location=Piscataway, NJL |publisher=TransAction Books |ref=harv}}
<!--Rushton & Jensen 2005-->
*{{Cite journal |doi=10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.235 |last2=Jensen |first2=Arthur R |last1=Rushton |first1=J. Philippe |year=2005 |title=Thirty Years of Research on Race Differences in Cognitive Ability |url=http://psychology.uwo.ca/faculty/rushtonpdfs/PPPL1.pdf |journal=Psychology, Public Policy, and Law |volume=11 |issue=2 |pages=246–8 |ref=harv |url-status=dead |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20151103215722/http://psychology.uwo.ca/faculty/rushtonpdfs/PPPL1.pdf |archivedate=2015-11-03 |citeseerx=10.1.1.186.102 }}
<!--Rushton & Jensen 2006-->
*{{cite journal |first1=J. Philippe |last1=Rushton |first2=Arthur R |last2=Jensen |year=2006 |title=The Totality of Available Evidence Shows the Race IQ Gap Still Remains |journal=Psychological Science |volume=17 |issue=10 |pages=921–2 |url=http://psychology.uwo.ca/faculty/rushtonpdfs/2006%20PSnew.pdf |pmid=17100794 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01803.x |ref=harv |url-status=dead |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20101105090928/http://psychology.uwo.ca/faculty/rushtonpdfs/2006%20PSnew.pdf |archivedate=2010-11-05 }}
<!--Rushton & Jensen 2010-->
*{{cite journal |first1=J. Philippe |last1=Rushton |first2=Arthur R |last2=Jensen |title=Race and IQ: A theory-based review of the research in Richard Nisbett's Intelligence and How to Get It |journal=The Open Psychology Journal |volume=3 |pages=9–35 |year=2010 |url=http://psychology.uwo.ca/faculty/rushtonpdfs/2010%20Review%20of%20Nisbett.pdf |doi=10.2174/1874350101003010009 |ref=harv |url-status=dead |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20110706212443/http://psychology.uwo.ca/faculty/rushtonpdfs/2010%20Review%20of%20Nisbett.pdf |archivedate=2011-07-06 }}
<!--Saloojee & Pettifor 2001-->
*{{cite journal |last=Saloojee |first=Haroon |last2=Pettifor |first2=John M |title=Iron deficiency and impaired child development |journal=BMJ |volume=323 |issue=7326 |pages=1377–8 |date = December 2001|pmid=11744547 |pmc=1121846 |doi=10.1136/bmj.323.7326.1377 |url=http://bmj.bmjjournals.com/cgi/content/full/323/7326/1377 |ref={{harvid|Saloojee|Pettifor|2001}} }}
<!-- Scarr & Weinberg 1990-->
*{{Cite book|last1=Scarr|first1=S.|last2=Weinberg|first2= R. A. |year=1990|chapter=The nature-nurture problem revisited: The Minnesota adoption studies|title=Methods of family research: Biographies of research projects| volume=1|pages=121–151|ref=harv}}
<!-- Scarr & Weinberg 1976-->
*{{cite journal | last1 = Scarr | first1 = S. | last2 = Weinberg | first2 = R. A. | year = 1976 | title = IQ test performance of black children adopted by White families | url = https://semanticscholar.org/paper/e6db5244b970f5390cf2486c7e65f14b0b59967c| journal = American Psychologist | volume = 31 | issue = 10| pages = 726–739 | doi=10.1037/0003-066x.31.10.726|ref=harv}}
<!--Schacter, Gilbert & Wegner 2007-->
*{{cite book |first=Daniel |last=Schacter |first2=Daniel |last2=Gilbert |first3=Daniel |last3=Wegner |year=2007 |title=Psychology |location=New York |publisher= Worth Publishing |isbn= 978-0-7167-5215-8 |ref=harv}}
<!--Schaefer 2008-->
*{{cite book |title=Encyclopedia of Race, Ethnicity and Society |chapter=Race |page=1091 |year=2008 |publisher=SAGE |volume=2 |editor-last=Schaefer |editor-first=Richard T. |ref={{harvid|Schaefer|2008}} }}
<!--Serpell 2000-->
*{{cite book |last=Serpell |first=Robert |chapter=Intelligence and Culture |title=Handbook of Intelligence |year=2000 |editor-last=Sternberg |editor-first=Robert J. |publisher=Cambridge University Press |isbn=978-0-521-59648-0 |pages=549–577 |ref=harv}}
<!--Sheppard & Vernon 2008-->
*{{cite journal|last1=Sheppard|first1=Leah D.|last2=Vernon|first2=Philip A.|title=Intelligence and speed of information-processing: A review of 50 years of research|journal=Personality and Individual Differences|date=February 2008|volume=44|issue=3|pages=535–551|doi=10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.015|ref=harv}}
<!--Shuttleworth-Edwards et al. 2004-->
*{{Cite book |title=Broken Genius: The Rise and Fall of William Shockley, Creator of the Electronic Age |last=Shurkin |first=Joel |year=2006 |publisher=Macmillan |location=London |isbn=978-1-4039-8815-7 |lay-url=http://www.popularscience.co.uk/reviews/rev291.htm |laydate=2 June 2013 |ref=harv }}
*{{cite journal |title=Cross-cultural effects on IQ test performance: a review and preliminary normative indications on WAIS-III test performance |last1= Shuttleworth-Edwards |first1=AB |last2=Kemp |first2= RD |last3=Rust |first3=AL |last4=Muirhead |first4=JG |last5=Hartman |first5=NP |last6=Radloff |first6=SE |journal=Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology |date=October 2004 |volume=26 |issue=7 |pages=903–20 |pmid=15742541 |ref={{harvid|Shuttleworth-Edwards et al.|2004}} |doi=10.1080/13803390490510824}}
<!--Smay & Armelagos 2000-->
*{{cite journal|last1=Smay|first1=Diana|last2=Armelagos|first2=George|title=Galileo Wept: A Critical Assessment of the Use of Race in Forensic Anthropology|journal=Transforming Anthropology|date=July 2000|volume=9|issue=2|pages=19–29|doi=10.1525/tran.2000.9.2.19|ref={{harvid|Smay|Armelagos|2000}}}}
<!--Snyderman & Rothman 1987-->
*{{cite journal |last1=Snyderman |first1= Mark |last2=Rothman |first2=Stanley |year=1987 |title=Survey of expert opinion on intelligence and aptitude testing |journal=American Psychologist |volume=42 |issue= 2 |pages=137–44 |ref=harv |doi=10.1037/0003-066x.42.2.137}}
<!--Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd 2005-->
*{{cite journal |first=Robert J |last=Sternberg |first2=Elena L |last2=Grigorenko |first3=Kenneth K |last3=Kidd |year=2005 |title=Intelligence, Race, and Genetics |journal=American Psychologist |volume=60 |issue=1 |pages=46–59 |url=http://medicine.yale.edu/labs/kidd/440.pdf |doi=10.1037/0003-066X.60.1.46 |pmid=15641921 |ref=harv |url-status=dead |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20110720180251/http://medicine.yale.edu/labs/kidd/440.pdf |archivedate=2011-07-20 |citeseerx=10.1.1.174.313 }}
<!--Sternberg & Grigorenko 2007-->
*{{Cite journal | last1 = Sternberg | first1 = R. J. | last2 = Grigorenko | first2 = E. L. | doi = 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00040.x | pmid = 26151963 | title = The Difficulty of Escaping Preconceptions in Writing an Article About the Difficulty of Escaping Preconceptions: Commentary on Hunt and Carlson (2007) | journal = Perspectives on Psychological Science | volume = 2 | issue = 2 | pages = 221–223 | year = 2007 | ref = harv}}
<!--Spiro 2009-->
*{{Cite book |title=Defending the Master Race: Conservation, Eugenics, and the Legacy of Madison Grant |last=Spiro |first=Jonathan P. |publisher=Univ. of Vermont Press |year=2009 |isbn=978-1-58465-715-6 |lay-url=http://www.upne.com/1-58465-715-4.html |laydate=29 September 2010 |ref=harv }}
<!--Suzuki & Aronson 2005-->
*{{cite journal |ref=harv |last1=Suzuki |first1=Lisa |last2=Aronson |first2=Joshua |title=The cultural malleability of intelligence and its impact on the racial/ethnic hierarchy. |journal=Psychology, Public Policy, and Law |date=2005 |volume=11 |issue=2 |pages=320–327 |doi=10.1037/1076-8971.11.2.320 |citeseerx=10.1.1.1022.3693 }}
<!--Tang et al. 2005-->
*<!--to identify citation in quotation in mainspace from Hunt & Carlson (2007)-->{{cite journal |first1=Hua |last1=Tang |first2=Tom |last2=Quertermous |first3=Beatriz |last3=Rodriguez |first4=Sharon L.R. |last4=Kardia |first5=Xiaofeng |last5=Zhu |first6=Andrew |last6=Brown |first7=James S. |last7=Pankow |first8=Michael A. |last8=Province |first9=Steven C. |last9=Hunt |first10=Eric |last10=Boerwinkle |first11=Nicholas J. |last11=Schork |first12=Neil J. |last12=Risch |date=February 2005 |title=Genetic structure, self-identified race/ethnicity, and confounding in case-control association studies |journal=American Journal of Human Genetics |volume= 76|issue=2 |pages=268–75 |pmid=15625622| doi=10.1086/427888 |ref={{harvid|Tang et al.|2005}} |pmc=1196372}}
<!--Templeton 2001 -->
*{{Citation | last = Templeton | first = A. R. | author-link = Alan R. Templeton | contribution = The Genetic and Evolutionary Significance of Human Races | editor-last = Fish | editor-first = J. M. | title = Race and Intelligence Separating Science from Myth | pages = 31–55 | publisher = Routledge | place = London | date = 2001 | isbn = 978-0805837575}}
<!--Thompson et al. 2001 -->
*{{cite journal |first1=Paul M. |last1=Thompson |first2=Tyrone D. |last2=Cannon |first3=Katherine L. |last3=Narr |first4=Theo |last4=van Erp |first5=Veli-Pekka |last5=Poutanen |first6=Matti |last6=Huttunen |first7=Jouko |last7=Lönnqvist |first8=Carl-Gustaf |last8=Standertskjöld-Nordenstam |first9=Jaakko |last9=Kaprio |first10=Mohammad |last10=Khaledy |first11=Rajneesh |last11=Dail |first12=Chris I. |last12=Zoumalan |first13=Arthur W. |last13=Toga |year=2001 |title=Genetic influences on brain structure |journal=Nature Neuroscience |volume=4 |issue=12 |pages=1253–58 |url=http://www.loni.ucla.edu/~thompson/MEDIA/NN/Nature_Neuro2001_genetics.pdf |doi=10.1038/nn758 |pmid=11694885 |ref={{harvid|Thompson et al.|2001}} |url-status=dead |archiveurl=https://web.archive.org/web/20110515085921/http://www.loni.ucla.edu/~thompson/MEDIA/NN/Nature_Neuro2001_genetics.pdf |archivedate=2011-05-15 }}
<!--Tizard et al. 1972 -->
*{{cite journal |last=Tizard |first=Barbara |last2=Cooperman |first2=Oliver |last3=Joseph |first3=Anne |last4=Tizard |first4=Jack |title=Environmental effects on language development: A study of young children in long-stay residential nurseries |journal=Child Development |volume=43 |pages=337–58 |date=June 1972 |doi=10.2307/1127540 |issue=2 |ref={{harvid|Tizard et al.|1972}} |jstor=1127540}}
<!--Tucker 2002-->
*{{Cite book |first=William H |last=Tucker |title=The Funding of Scientific Racism: Wickliffe Draper and the Pioneer Fund |publisher=University of Illinois Press |isbn=978-0-252-02762-8 |year=2002 |ref=harv}}
<!--Verney et al. 2005-->
*{{cite journal |ref={{harvid|Verney et al.|2005}} |last1=Verney |first1=Steven P. |last2=Granholm |first2=Eric |last3=Marshall |first3=Sandra P. |last4=Malcarne |first4=Vanessa L. |last5=Saccuzzo |first5=Dennis P. |title=Culture-Fair Cognitive Ability Assessment |journal=Assessment |date=26 July 2016 |volume=12 |issue=3 |pages=303–319 |doi=10.1177/1073191105276674 |pmid=16123251 }}
<!--Vincent 1991-->
*{{cite journal |last1=Vincent |first1=Ken R. |title=Black/white IQ differences: Does age make the difference? |journal=Journal of Clinical Psychology |date=March 1991 |volume=47 |issue=2 |pages=266–270 |doi=10.1002/1097-4679(199103)47:2<266::aid-jclp2270470213>3.0.co;2-s |ref={{harvid|Vincent|1991}} |pmid=2030133 }}
<!--Wade 2011 -->
*{{cite news |first=Nicholas |last=Wade |url=https://www.nytimes.com/2011/06/14/science/14skull.html?_r=1&ref=intelligence |title=Scientists Measure the Accuracy of a Racism Claim |newspaper=New York Times |date=13 June 2011 |ref={{harvid|New York Times|2011}}}}
<!--Weinbert, Scarr & Waldman 1992-->
*{{cite journal |last1=Weinberg |first1=Richard A. |last2=Scarr |first2=Sandra |last3=Waldman |first3=Irwin D. |title=The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study: A Follow-Up of IQ Test Performance at Adolescence |journal=Intelligence |volume=16 |issue=1 |pages=117–35 |year=1992 |doi=10.1016/0160-2896(92)90028-P |ref=harv }}
<!--Wicherts, Borsboom & Dolan 2010 -->
*{{cite journal |title=Why national IQs do not support evolutionary theories of intelligence |first=Jelte M |last=Wicherts |first2=Denny |last2=Borsboom |first3=Conor V |last3=Dolan |journal=Personality and Individual Differences |volume=48 |issue=2 |year=2010 |pages=91–6 |ref=harv |doi=10.1016/j.paid.2009.05.028}}
<!--Wicherts, Borsboom & Dolan 2010b -->
*{{cite journal|last1=Wicherts|first1=Jelte M.|last2=Borsboom|first2=Denny|last3=Dolan|first3=Conor V.|title=Evolution, brain size, and the national IQ of peoples around 3000 years B.C|journal=Personality and Individual Differences|year=2010b|volume=48|issue=2|pages=104–106|doi=10.1016/j.paid.2009.08.020|ref=harv}}
<!--Wicherts, Dolan, Carlson, & van der Maas 2010-->
*{{cite journal|last1=Wicherts|first1=Jelte M.|last2=Dolan|first2=Conor V.|last3=Carlson|first3=Jerry S.|last4=van der Maas|first4=Han L.J.|title=Raven's test performance of sub-Saharan Africans: Average performance, psychometric properties, and the Flynn Effect|journal=Learning and Individual Differences|date=June 2010|volume=20|issue=3|pages=135–151|doi=10.1016/j.lindif.2009.12.001|ref=harv}}
<!--Wicherts, Dolan & van der Maas 2010-->
*{{cite journal |ref=harv |last1=Wicherts |first1=Jelte M. |last2=Dolan |first2=Conor V. |last3=van der Maas |first3=Han L.J. |title=A systematic literature review of the average IQ of sub-Saharan Africans |journal=Intelligence |date=January 2010 |volume=38 |issue=1 |pages=1–20 |doi=10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.002 }}
<!--Witelson, Beresh & Kigar 2006 -->
*{{cite journal |ref=harv |last1=Witelson |first1=S. F. |last2=Beresh |first2=H. |last3=Kigar |first3=D. L. |title=Intelligence and brain size in 100 postmortem brains: sex, lateralization and age factors |journal=Brain |date=February 2006 |volume=129 |issue=2 |pages=386–398 |doi=10.1093/brain/awh696 |pmid=16339797 }}
<!--Wooldridge 1995-->
*{{Cite book |first= Adrian |last=Wooldridge |title=Measuring the Mind: Education and Psychology in England c. 1860-c. 1990 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |year=1995 |isbn=978-0-521-39515-1 |ref=harv}}
<!--Wroe 2008-->
*{{cite book |first=Andrew |last=Wroe |title=The Republican Party and Immigration Politics: from Proposition 187 to George W. Bush |publisher=Macmillan |year=2008 |page=294 |isbn= 978-0-230-60053-9 |ref=harv}}
<!--Zinkstok et al. 2007-->
*{{cite journal | last1 = Zinkstok | first1 = Janneke R | last2 =De Wilde| first2 = Odette | last3 = Van Amelsvoort| year = 2007 | first3 = Therese AMJ | last4 = Tanck | first4 = Michael W | last5 = Baas | first5 = Frank | last6 = Linszen | first6 = Don H | title = Association between the DTNBP1 gene and intelligence: a case-control study in young patients with schizophrenia and related disorders and unaffected siblings | journal = Behavioral and Brain Functions | volume = 3 | page = 19 | doi = 10.1186/1744-9081-3-19 | pmc = 1864987 | pmid=17445278 |ref={{harvid|Zinkstok et al.|2007}} }}

{{refend}} {{refend}}

==External links==
*, containing papers arguing various perspectives about race and intelligence.
* about whether the black/white IQ gap is shrinking or staying the same&nbsp;– November 2006.
*, a TV documentary made by the UK's ] public television station, featuring ]-British journalist ], October 2009.


{{Human intelligence topics}} {{Human intelligence topics}}
Line 568: Line 365:


{{DEFAULTSORT:Race And Intelligence}} {{DEFAULTSORT:Race And Intelligence}}
] ]
] ]
] ]

Latest revision as of 06:37, 5 January 2025

Discussions and claims of differences in intelligence along racial lines

Race
History
Society
Race and...
By location
Related topics

Discussions of race and intelligence – specifically regarding claims of differences in intelligence along racial lines – have appeared in both popular science and academic research since the modern concept of race was first introduced. With the inception of IQ testing in the early 20th century, differences in average test performance between racial groups have been observed, though these differences have fluctuated and in many cases steadily decreased over time. Complicating the issue, modern science has concluded that race is a socially constructed phenomenon rather than a biological reality, and there exist various conflicting definitions of intelligence. In particular, the validity of IQ testing as a metric for human intelligence is disputed. Today, the scientific consensus is that genetics does not explain differences in IQ test performance between groups, and that observed differences are environmental in origin.

Pseudoscientific claims of inherent differences in intelligence between races have played a central role in the history of scientific racism. The first tests showing differences in IQ scores between different population groups in the United States were the tests of United States Army recruits in World War I. In the 1920s, groups of eugenics lobbyists argued that these results demonstrated that African Americans and certain immigrant groups were of inferior intellect to Anglo-Saxon white people, and that this was due to innate biological differences. In turn, they used such beliefs to justify policies of racial segregation. However, other studies soon appeared, contesting these conclusions and arguing that the Army tests had not adequately controlled for environmental factors, such as socioeconomic and educational inequality between the groups.

Later observations of phenomena such as the Flynn effect and disparities in access to prenatal care highlighted ways in which environmental factors affect group IQ differences. In recent decades, as understanding of human genetics has advanced, claims of inherent differences in intelligence between races have been broadly rejected by scientists on both theoretical and empirical grounds.

History of the controversy

Main article: History of the race and intelligence controversy See also: Scientific racism
Autodidact and abolitionist Frederick Douglass (1817–1895) served as a high-profile counterexample to myths of black intellectual inferiority.

Claims of differences in intelligence between races have been used to justify colonialism, slavery, racism, social Darwinism, and racial eugenics. Claims of intellectual inferiority were used to justify British wars and colonial campaigns in Asia. Racial thinkers such as Arthur de Gobineau in France relied crucially on the assumption that black people were innately inferior to white people in developing their ideologies of white supremacy. Even Enlightenment thinkers such as Thomas Jefferson, a slave owner, believed black people to be innately inferior to white people in physique and intellect. At the same time in the United States, prominent examples of African-American genius such the autodidact and abolitionist Frederick Douglass, the pioneering sociologist W. E. B. Du Bois, and the poet Paul Laurence Dunbar stood as high-profile counterexamples to widespread stereotypes of black intellectual inferiority. In Britain, Japan's military victory over Russia in the Russo-Japanese War began to reverse negative stereotypes of "oriental" inferiority.

Alfred Binet (1857–1911), inventor of the first intelligence test

Early IQ testing

The first practical intelligence test, the Binet-Simon Intelligence Test, was developed between 1905 and 1908 by Alfred Binet and Théodore Simon in France for school placement of children. Binet warned that results from his test should not be assumed to measure innate intelligence or used to label individuals permanently. Binet's test was translated into English and revised in 1916 by Lewis Terman (who introduced IQ scoring for the test results) and published under the name Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales. In 1916 Terman wrote that Mexican-Americans, African-Americans, and Native Americans have a mental "dullness seems to be racial, or at least inherent in the family stocks from which they come."

The US Army used a different set of tests developed by Robert Yerkes to evaluate draftees for World War I. Based on the Army's data, prominent psychologists and eugenicists such as Henry H. Goddard, Harry H. Laughlin, and Princeton professor Carl Brigham wrote that people from southern and eastern Europe were less intelligent than native-born Americans or immigrants from the Nordic countries, and that black Americans were less intelligent than white Americans. The results were widely publicized by a lobby of anti-immigration activists, including the conservationist and theorist of scientific racism Madison Grant, who considered the so-called Nordic race to be superior, but under threat because of immigration by "inferior breeds." In his influential work, A Study of American Intelligence, psychologist Carl Brigham used the results of the Army tests to argue for a stricter immigration policy, limiting immigration to countries considered to belong to the "Nordic race".

In the 1920s, some US states enacted eugenic laws, such as Virginia's 1924 Racial Integrity Act, which established the one-drop rule (of 'racial purity') as law. Many scientists reacted negatively to eugenicist claims linking abilities and moral character to racial or genetic ancestry. They pointed to the contribution of environment (such as speaking English as a second language) to test results. By the mid-1930s, many psychologists in the US had adopted the view that environmental and cultural factors played a dominant role in IQ test results. The psychologist Carl Brigham repudiated his own earlier arguments, explaining that he had come to realize that the tests were not a measure of innate intelligence.

Discussions of the issue in the United States, especially in the writings of Madison Grant, influenced German Nazi claims that the "Nordics" were a "master race." As American public sentiment shifted against the Germans, claims of racial differences in intelligence increasingly came to be regarded as problematic. Anthropologists such as Franz Boas, Ruth Benedict, and Gene Weltfish did much to demonstrate that claims about racial hierarchies of intelligence were unscientific. Nonetheless, a powerful eugenics and segregation lobby funded largely by textile-magnate Wickliffe Draper continued to use intelligence studies as an argument for eugenics, segregation, and anti-immigration legislation.

The Pioneer Fund and The Bell Curve

As the desegregation of the American South gained traction in the 1950s, debate about black intelligence resurfaced. Audrey Shuey, funded by Draper's Pioneer Fund, published a new analysis of Yerkes' tests, concluding that black people really were of inferior intellect to white people. This study was used by segregationists to argue that it was to the advantage of black children to be educated separately from the superior white children. In the 1960s, the debate was revived when William Shockley publicly defended the view that black children were innately unable to learn as well as white children. Arthur Jensen expressed similar opinions in his Harvard Educational Review article, "How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement?," which questioned the value of compensatory education for African-American children. He suggested that poor educational performance in such cases reflected an underlying genetic cause rather than lack of stimulation at home or other environmental factors.

Another revival of public debate followed the appearance of The Bell Curve (1994), a book by Richard Herrnstein and Charles Murray that supported the general viewpoint of Jensen. A statement in support of Herrnstein and Murray titled "Mainstream Science on Intelligence," was published in The Wall Street Journal with 52 signatures. The Bell Curve also led to critical responses in a statement titled "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns" of the American Psychological Association and in several books, including The Bell Curve Debate (1995), Inequality by Design (1996) and a second edition of The Mismeasure of Man (1996) by Stephen Jay Gould.

Some of the authors proposing genetic explanations for group differences have received funding from the Pioneer Fund, which was headed by J. Philippe Rushton until his death in 2012. Arthur Jensen, who jointly with Rushton published a 2005 review article arguing that the difference in average IQs between blacks and whites is partly due to genetics, received $1.1 million in grants from the Pioneer Fund. According to Ashley Montagu, "The University of California's Arthur Jensen, cited twenty-three times in The Bell Curve's bibliography, is the book's principal authority on the intellectual inferiority of blacks."

The Southern Poverty Law Center lists the Pioneer Fund as a hate group, citing the fund's history, its funding of race and intelligence research, and its connections with racist individuals. Other researchers have criticized the Pioneer Fund for promoting scientific racism, eugenics and white supremacy.

Conceptual issues

Intelligence and IQ

Main articles: Human intelligence, Intelligence quotient, and G factor (psychometrics)

The concept of intelligence and the degree to which intelligence is measurable are matters of debate. There is no consensus about how to define intelligence; nor is it universally accepted that it is something that can be meaningfully measured by a single figure. A recurring criticism is that different societies value and promote different kinds of skills and that the concept of intelligence is therefore culturally variable and cannot be measured by the same criteria in different societies. Consequently, some critics argue that it makes no sense to propose relationships between intelligence and other variables.

Correlations between scores on various types of IQ tests led English psychologist Charles Spearman to propose in 1904 the existence of an underlying factor, which he referred to as "g" or "general intelligence", a trait which is supposed to be innate. Another proponent of this view is Arthur Jensen. This view, however, has been contradicted by a number of studies showing that education and changes in environment can significantly improve IQ test results.

Other psychometricians have argued that, whether or not there is such a thing as a general intelligence factor, performance on tests relies crucially on knowledge acquired through prior exposure to the types of tasks that such tests contain. This means that comparisons of test scores between persons with widely different life experiences and cognitive habits do not reveal their relative innate potentials.

Race

Main articles: Race (human categorization) and Race and genetics

The consensus view among geneticists, biologists and anthropologists is that race is a sociopolitical phenomenon rather than a biological one, a view supported by considerable genetics research. The current mainstream view is that race is a social construction based on folk ideologies that construct groups based on social disparities and superficial physical characteristics. A 2023 consensus report from the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine stated: "In humans, race is a socially constructed designation, a misleading and harmful surrogate for population genetic differences, and has a long history of being incorrectly identified as the major genetic reason for phenotypic differences between groups."

The concept of human "races" as natural and separate divisions within the human species has also been rejected by the American Anthropological Association. The official position of the AAA, adopted in 1998, is that advances in scientific knowledge have made it "clear that human populations are not unambiguous, clearly demarcated, biologically distinct groups" and that "any attempt to establish lines of division among biological populations both arbitrary and subjective." A more recent statement from the American Association of Physical Anthropologists (2019) declares that "Race does not provide an accurate representation of human biological variation. It was never accurate in the past, and it remains inaccurate when referencing contemporary human populations. Humans are not divided biologically into distinct continental types or racial genetic clusters."

Anthropologists such as C. Loring Brace, the philosophers Jonathan Kaplan and Rasmus Winther, and the geneticist Joseph Graves, have argued that the cluster structure of genetic data is dependent on the initial hypotheses of the researcher and the influence of these hypotheses on the choice of populations to sample. When one samples continental groups, the clusters become continental, but if one had chosen other sampling patterns, the clustering would be different. Weiss and Fullerton have noted that if one sampled only Icelanders, Mayans and Maoris, three distinct clusters would form and all other populations could be described as being clinally composed of admixtures of Maori, Icelandic and Mayan genetic materials. Kaplan and Winther conclude that while racial groups are characterized by different allele frequencies, this does not mean that racial classification is a natural taxonomy of the human species, because multiple other genetic patterns can be found in human populations that crosscut racial distinctions. Moreover, the genomic data underdetermines whether one wishes to see subdivisions (i.e., splitters) or a continuum (i.e., lumpers). Under Kaplan and Winther's view, racial groupings are objective social constructions (see Mills 1998) that have conventional biological reality only insofar as the categories are chosen and constructed for pragmatic scientific reasons. Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd (2005) argue that the social construction of race derives not from any valid scientific basis but rather "from people's desire to classify."

In studies of human intelligence, race is almost always determined using self-reports rather than analyses of genetic characteristics. According to psychologist David Rowe, self-report is the preferred method for racial classification in studies of racial differences because classification based on genetic markers alone ignore the "cultural, behavioral, sociological, psychological, and epidemiological variables" that distinguish racial groups. Hunt and Carlson disagreed, writing that "Nevertheless, self-identification is a surprisingly reliable guide to genetic composition," citing a study by Tang et al. (2005). Sternberg and Grigorenko disputed Hunt and Carlson's interpretation of Tang's results as supporting the view that racial divisions are biological; rather, "Tang et al.'s point was that ancient geographic ancestry rather than current residence is associated with self-identification and not that such self-identification provides evidence for the existence of biological race."

Group differences

The study of human intelligence is one of the most controversial topics in psychology, in part because of difficulty reaching agreement about the meaning of intelligence and objections to the assumption that intelligence can be meaningfully measured by IQ tests. Claims that there are innate differences in intelligence between racial and ethnic groups—which go back at least to the 19th century—have been criticized for relying on specious assumptions and research methods and for serving as an ideological framework for discrimination and racism.

In a 2012 study of tests of different components of intelligence, Hampshire et al. expressed disagreement with the view of Jensen and Rushton that genetic factors must play a role in IQ differences between races, stating that "it remains unclear ... whether population differences in intelligence test scores are driven by heritable factors or by other correlated demographic variables such as socioeconomic status, education level, and motivation. More relevantly, it is questionable whether relate to a unitary intelligence factor, as opposed to a bias in testing paradigms toward particular components of a more complex intelligence construct." According to Jackson and Weidman,

There are a number of reasons why the genetic argument for race differences in intelligence has not won many adherents in the scientific community. First, even taken on its own terms, the case made by Jensen and his followers did not hold up to scrutiny. Second, the rise of population genetics undercut the claims for a genetic cause of intelligence. Third, the new understanding of institutional racism offered a better explanation for the existence of differences in IQ scores between the races.

Test scores

Main article: Achievement gap in the United States

In the United States, Asians on average score higher than White people, who tend to score higher than Hispanics, who tend to score higher than African Americans. Much greater variation in IQ scores exists within each ethnic group than between them. A 2001 meta-analysis of the results of 6,246,729 participants tested for cognitive ability or aptitude found a difference in average scores between black people and white people of 1.1 standard deviations. Consistent results were found for college and university application tests such as the Scholastic Aptitude Test (N = 2.4 million) and Graduate Record Examination (N = 2.3 million), as well as for tests of job applicants in corporate settings (N = 0.5 million) and in the military (N = 0.4 million).

In response to the controversial 1994 book The Bell Curve, the American Psychological Association (APA) formed a task-force of eleven experts, which issued a report "Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns" in 1996. Regarding group differences, the report reaffirmed the consensus that differences within groups are much wider than differences between groups, and that claims of ethnic differences in intelligence should be scrutinized carefully, as such claims had been used to justify racial discrimination. The report also acknowledged problems with the racial categories used, as these categories are neither consistently applied, nor homogeneous (see Race and ethnicity in the United States).

In the UK, some African groups have higher average educational attainment and standardized test scores than the overall population. In 2010–2011, white British pupils were 2.3% less likely to have gained 5 A*–C grades at GCSE than the national average, whereas the likelihood was 21.8% above average for those of Nigerian origin, 5.5% above average for those of Ghanaian origin, and 1.4% above average for those of Sierra Leonian origin. For the two other African ethnic groups on which data was available, the likelihood was 23.7% below average for those of Somali origin and 35.3% below average for those of Congolese origin. In 2014, Black-African pupils of 11 language groups were more likely to pass Key Stage 2 Maths 4+ in England than the national average. Overall, the average pass rate by ethnicity was 86.5% for white British (N = 395,787), whereas it was 85.6% for Black-Africans (N = 18,497). Nevertheless, several Black-African language groups, including Yoruba, Igbo, Hausa, Akan, Ga, Swahili, Edo, Ewe, Amharic speakers, and English-speaking Africans, each had an average pass rate above the white British average (total N = 9,314), with the Hausa, Igbo, Yoruba, and Amhara having averages above 90% (N = 2,071). In 2017–2018, the percentage of pupils getting a strong pass (grade 5 or above) in the English and maths GCSE (in Key Stage 4) was 42.7% for whites (N = 396,680) and 44.3% for Black-Africans (N = 18,358).

Flynn effect and the closing gap

Main article: Flynn effect

The 'Flynn effect' — a term coined after researcher James R. Flynn — refers to the substantial rise in raw IQ test scores observed in many parts of the world during the 20th century. In the United States, the increase was continuous and approximately linear from the earliest years of testing to about 1998 when the gains stopped and some tests even showed decreasing test scores. For example, the average scores of black people on some IQ tests in 1995 were the same as the scores of white people in 1945. As one pair of academics phrased it, "the typical African American today probably has a slightly higher IQ than the grandparents of today's average white American."

Flynn himself argued that the dramatic changes having taken place between one just generation and the next pointed strongly at an environmental explanation, and that it is highly unlikely that genetic factors could have accounted for the increasing scores. The Flynn effect, along with Flynn's analysis, continues to hold significance in the context of the black/white IQ gap debate, demonstrating the potential for environmental factors to influence IQ test scores by as much as 1 standard deviation, a scale of change that had previously been doubted.

A distinct but related observation has been the gradual narrowing of the American black-white IQ gap in the last decades of the 20th century, as black test-takers increased their average scores relative to white test-takers. For instance, Vincent reported in 1991 that the black–white IQ gap was decreasing among children, but that it was remaining constant among adults. Similarly, a 2006 study by Dickens and Flynn estimated that the difference between mean scores of black people and white people closed by about 5 or 6 IQ points between 1972 and 2002, a reduction of about one-third. In the same period, the educational achievement disparity also diminished. Reviews by Flynn and Dickens, Mackintosh, and Nisbett et al. accept the gradual closing of the gap as a fact. Flynn and Dickens summarize this trend, stating, "The constancy of the Black-White IQ gap is a myth and therefore cannot be cited as evidence that the racial IQ gap is genetic in origin."

Environmental factors

Health and nutrition

Main article: Impact of health on intelligence
Percentage of children aged 1–5 with blood lead levels at least 10 μg/dL. Black and Hispanic children have much higher levels than white children. A 10 μg/dL increase in blood lead at 24 months is associated with a 5.8-point decline in IQ. Although the Geometric Mean Blood Lead Levels (GM BLL) are declining, a CDC report (2002) states that: "However, the GM BLL for non-Hispanic black children remains higher than that for Mexican-American and non-Hispanic white children, indicating that differences in risk for exposure still persist."

Environmental factors including childhood lead exposure, low rates of breast feeding, and poor nutrition are significantly correlated with poor cognitive development and functioning. For example, childhood exposure to lead — associated with homes in poorer areas — correlates with an average IQ drop of 7 points, and iodine deficiency causes a decline, on average, of 12 IQ points. Such impairments may sometimes be permanent, but in some cases they be partially or wholly compensated for by later growth.

The first two years of life are critical for malnutrition, the consequences of which are often irreversible and include poor cognitive development, educability, and future economic productivity. Mackintosh points out that, for American black people, infant mortality is about twice as high as for white people, and low birth weight is twice as prevalent. At the same time, white mothers are twice as likely to breastfeed their infants, and breastfeeding is directly correlated with IQ for low-birth-weight infants. In this way, a wide number of health-related factors which influence IQ are unequally distributed between the two groups.

The Copenhagen consensus in 2004 stated that lack of both iodine and iron has been implicated in impaired brain development, and this can affect enormous numbers of people: it is estimated that one-third of the total global population is affected by iodine deficiency. In developing countries, it is estimated that 40% of children aged four and under have anaemia because of insufficient iron in their diets.

Other scholars have found that simply the standard of nutrition has a significant effect on population intelligence, and that the Flynn effect may be caused by increasing nutrition standards across the world. James Flynn has himself argued against this view.

Some recent research has argued that the retardation caused in brain development by infectious diseases, many of which are more prevalent in non-white populations, may be an important factor in explaining the differences in IQ between different regions of the world. The findings of this research, showing the correlation between IQ, race and infectious diseases was also shown to apply to the IQ gap in the US, suggesting that this may be an important environmental factor.

A 2013 meta-analysis by the World Health Organization found that, after controlling for maternal IQ, breastfeeding was associated with IQ gains of 2.19 points. The authors suggest that this relationship is causal but state that the practical significance of this gain is debatable; however, they highlight one study suggesting an association between breastfeeding and academic performance in Brazil, where "breastfeeding duration does not present marked variability by socioeconomic position." Colen and Ramey (2014) similarly find that controlling for sibling comparisons within families, rather than between families, reduces the correlation between breastfeeding status and WISC IQ scores by nearly a third, but further find the relationship between breastfeeding duration and WISC IQ scores to be insignificant. They suggest that "much of the beneficial long-term effects typically attributed to breastfeeding, per se, may primarily be due to selection pressures into infant feeding practices along key demographic characteristics such as race and socioeconomic status." Reichman estimates that no more than 3 to 4% of the black–white IQ gap can be explained by black–white disparities in low birth weight.

Education

Several studies have proposed that a large part of the gap in IQ test performance can be attributed to differences in quality of education. Racial discrimination in education has been proposed as one possible cause of differences in educational quality between races. According to a paper by Hala Elhoweris, Kagendo Mutua, Negmeldin Alsheikh and Pauline Holloway, teachers' referral decisions for students to participate in gifted and talented educational programs were influenced in part by the students' ethnicity.

The Abecedarian Early Intervention Project, an intensive early childhood education project, was also able to bring about an average IQ gain of 4.4 points at age 21 in the black children who participated in it compared to controls. Arthur Jensen agreed that the Abecedarian project demonstrated that education can have a significant effect on IQ, but also declared his view that no educational program thus far had been able to reduce the black–white IQ gap by more than a third, and that differences in education are thus unlikely to be its only cause.

A series of studies by Joseph Fagan and Cynthia Holland measured the effect of prior exposure to the kind of cognitive tasks posed in IQ tests on test performance. Assuming that the IQ gap was the result of lower exposure to tasks using the cognitive functions usually found in IQ tests among African American test takers, they prepared a group of African Americans in this type of tasks before taking an IQ test. The researchers found that there was no subsequent difference in performance between the African-Americans and white test takers. Daley and Onwuegbuzie conclude that Fagan and Holland demonstrate that "differences in knowledge between black people and white people for intelligence test items can be erased when equal opportunity is provided for exposure to the information to be tested". A similar argument is made by David Marks who argues that IQ differences correlate well with differences in literacy suggesting that developing literacy skills through education causes an increase in IQ test performance.

A 2003 study found that two variables—stereotype threat and the degree of educational attainment of children's fathers—partially explained the black–white gap in cognitive ability test scores, undermining the hereditarian view that they stemmed from immutable genetic factors.

Socioeconomic environment

Different aspects of the socioeconomic environment in which children are raised have been shown to correlate with part of the IQ gap, but they do not account for the entire gap. According to a 2006 review, these factors account for slightly less than half of one standard deviation.

Other research has focused on different causes of variation within low socioeconomic status (SES) and high SES groups. In the US, among low SES groups, genetic differences account for a smaller proportion of the variance in IQ than among high SES populations. Such effects are predicted by the bioecological hypothesis—that genotypes are transformed into phenotypes through nonadditive synergistic effects of the environment. Nisbett et al. (2012a) suggest that high SES individuals are more likely to be able to develop their full biological potential, whereas low SES individuals are likely to be hindered in their development by adverse environmental conditions. The same review also points out that adoption studies generally are biased towards including only high and high middle SES adoptive families, meaning that they will tend to overestimate average genetic effects. They also note that studies of adoption from lower-class homes to middle-class homes have shown that such children experience a 12 to 18 point gain in IQ relative to children who remain in low SES homes. A 2015 study found that environmental factors (namely, family income, maternal education, maternal verbal ability/knowledge, learning materials in the home, parenting factors, child birth order, and child birth weight) accounted for the black–white gap in cognitive ability test scores.

Test bias

A number of studies have reached the conclusion that IQ tests may be biased against certain groups. The validity and reliability of IQ scores obtained from outside the United States and Europe have been questioned, in part because of the inherent difficulty of comparing IQ scores between cultures. Several researchers have argued that cultural differences limit the appropriateness of standard IQ tests in non-industrialized communities.

A 1996 report by the American Psychological Association states that intelligence can be difficult to compare across cultures, and notes that differing familiarity with test materials can produce substantial differences in test results; it also says that tests are accurate predictors of future achievement for black and white Americans, and are in that sense unbiased. The view that tests accurately predict future educational attainment is reinforced by Nicholas Mackintosh in his 1998 book IQ and Human Intelligence, and by a 1999 literature review by Brown, Reynolds & Whitaker (1999).

James R. Flynn, surveying studies on the topic, notes that the weight and presence of many test questions depends on what sorts of information and modes of thinking are culturally valued.

Stereotype threat and minority status

Main article: Stereotype threat

Stereotype threat is the fear that one's behavior will confirm an existing stereotype of a group with which one identifies or by which one is defined; this fear may in turn lead to an impairment of performance. Testing situations that highlight the fact that intelligence is being measured tend to lower the scores of individuals from racial-ethnic groups who already score lower on average or are expected to score lower. Stereotype threat conditions cause larger than expected IQ differences among groups. Psychometrician Nicholas Mackintosh considers that there is little doubt that the effects of stereotype threat contribute to the IQ gap between black people and white people.

A large number of studies have shown that systemically disadvantaged minorities, such as the African American minority of the United States, generally perform worse in the educational system and in intelligence tests than the majority groups or less disadvantaged minorities such as immigrant or "voluntary" minorities. The explanation of these findings may be that children of caste-like minorities, due to the systemic limitations of their prospects of social advancement, do not have "effort optimism", i.e. they do not have the confidence that acquiring the skills valued by majority society, such as those skills measured by IQ tests, is worthwhile. They may even deliberately reject certain behaviors that are seen as "acting white." Research published in 1997 indicates that part of the black–white gap in cognitive ability test scores is due to racial differences in test motivation.

Some researchers have suggested that stereotype threat should not be interpreted as a factor in real-life performance gaps, and have raised the possibility of publication bias. Other critics have focused on correcting what they claim are misconceptions of early studies showing a large effect. However, numerous meta-analyses and systematic reviews have shown significant evidence for the effects of stereotype threat, though the phenomenon defies over-simplistic characterization. For instance, one meta-analysis found that with female subjects "subtle threat-activating cues produced the largest effect, followed by blatant and moderately explicit cues" while with minorities "moderately explicit stereotype threat-activating cues produced the largest effect, followed by blatant and subtle cues".

Some researchers have argued that studies of stereotype threat may in fact systematically under-represent its effects, since such studies measure "only that portion of psychological threat that research has identified and remedied. To the extent that unidentified or unremedied psychological threats further undermine performance, the results underestimate the bias."

Research into possible genetic factors

See also: Heritability of IQ

Although IQ differences between individuals have been shown to have a large hereditary component, it does not follow that mean group-level disparities (between-group differences) in IQ necessarily have a genetic basis. The scientific consensus is that there is no evidence for a genetic component behind IQ differences between racial groups. Growing evidence indicates that environmental factors, not genetic ones, explain the racial IQ gap.

Genetics of race and intelligence

Main article: Race and genetics

Geneticist Alan R. Templeton argued that the question about the possible genetic effects on the test score gap is muddled by the general focus on "race" rather than on populations defined by gene frequency or by geographical proximity, and by the general insistence on phrasing the question in terms of heritability. Templeton pointed out that racial groups neither represent sub-species nor distinct evolutionary lineages, and that therefore there is no basis for making claims about the general intelligence of races. He argued that, for these reasons, the search for possible genetic influences on the black–white test score gap is a priori flawed, because there is no genetic material shared by all Africans or by all Europeans. Mackintosh (2011), on the other hand, argued that by using genetic cluster analysis to correlate gene frequencies with continental populations it might be possible to show that African populations have a higher frequency of certain genetic variants that contribute to differences in average intelligence. Such a hypothetical situation could hold without all Africans carrying the same genes or belonging to a single evolutionary lineage. According to Mackintosh, a biological basis for the observed gap in IQ test performance thus cannot be ruled out on a priori grounds.

Hunt (2010, p. 447) noted that "no genes related to difference in cognitive skills have across the various racial and ethnic groups have ever been discovered. The argument for genetic differences has been carried forward largely by circumstantial evidence. Of course, tomorrow afternoon genetic mechanisms producing racial and ethnic differences in intelligence might be discovered, but there have been a lot of investigations, and tomorrow has not come for quite some time now." Mackintosh (2011, p. 344) concurred, noting that while several environmental factors have been shown to influence the IQ gap, the evidence for a genetic influence has been negligible. A 2012 review by Nisbett et al. (2012a) concluded that the entire IQ gap can be explained by known environmental factors, and Mackintosh found this view to be plausible.

More recent research attempting to identify genetic loci associated with individual-level differences in IQ has yielded promising results, which led the editorial board of Nature to issue a statement differentiating this research from the "racist" pseudoscience which it acknowledged has dogged intelligence research since its inception. It characterized the idea of genetically determined differences in intelligence between races as definitively false. Analysis of polygenic scores sampled from the 1000 Genomes Project has likewise found no evidence that intelligence was under diversifying selection in Africans and Europeans, suggesting that genetic differences make up a negligible component of the observed Black-White gap in IQ.

Heritability within and between groups

An environmental factor that varies between groups but not within groups can cause group differences in a trait that is otherwise 100 percent heritable.

Twin studies of intelligence have reported high heritability values. However, these studies have been criticized for being based on questionable assumptions. When used in the context of human behavior genetics, the term "heritability" can be misleading, as it does not necessarily convey information about the relative importance of genetic or environmental factors on the development of a given trait, nor does it convey the extent to which that trait is genetically determined. Arguments in support of a genetic explanation of racial differences in IQ are sometimes fallacious. For instance, hereditarians have sometimes cited the failure of known environmental factors to account for such differences, or the high heritability of intelligence within races, as evidence that racial differences in IQ are genetic.

Psychometricians have found that intelligence is substantially heritable within populations, with 30–50% of variance in IQ scores in early childhood being attributable to genetic factors in analyzed US populations, increasing to 75–80% by late adolescence. In biology heritability is defined as the ratio of variation attributable to genetic differences in an observable trait to the trait's total observable variation. The heritability of a trait describes the proportion of variation in the trait that is attributable to genetic factors within a particular population. A heritability of 1 indicates that variation correlates fully with genetic variation and a heritability of 0 indicates that there is no correlation between the trait and genes at all. In psychological testing, heritability tends to be understood as the degree of correlation between the results of a test taker and those of their biological parents. However, since high heritability is simply a correlation between child and parents, it does not describe the causes of heritability which in humans can be either genetic or environmental.

Therefore, a high heritability measure does not imply that a trait is genetic or unchangeable. In addition, environmental factors that affect all group members equally will not be measured by heritability, and the heritability of a trait may also change over time in response to changes in the distribution of genetic and environmental factors. High heritability does not imply that all of the heritability is genetically determined; rather, it can also be due to environmental differences that affect only a certain genetically defined group (indirect heritability).

The figure to the right demonstrates how heritability works. In each of the two gardens the difference between tall and short cornstalks is 100% heritable, as cornstalks that are genetically disposed for growing tall will become taller than those without this disposition. But the difference in height between the cornstalks to the left and those on the right is 100% environmental, as it is due to different nutrients being supplied to the two gardens. Hence, the causes of differences within a group and between groups may not be the same, even when looking at traits that are highly heritable.

Spearman's hypothesis

Main article: Spearman's hypothesis

Spearman's hypothesis states that the magnitude of the black–white difference in tests of cognitive ability depends entirely or mainly on the extent to which a test measures general mental ability, or g. The hypothesis was first formalized by Arthur Jensen, who devised the statistical "method of correlated vectors" to test it. If Spearman's hypothesis holds true, then the cognitive tasks that have the highest g-load are the tasks in which the gap between black and white test takers are greatest. Jensen and Rushton took this to show that the cause of g and the cause of the gap are the same—in their view, genetic differences.

Mackintosh (2011, pp. 338–39) acknowledges that Jensen and Rushton showed a modest correlation between g-loading, heritability, and the test score gap, but does not agree that this demonstrates a genetic origin of the gap. Mackintosh argues that it is exactly those tests that Rushton and Jensen consider to have the highest g-loading and heritability, such as the Wechsler test, that have seen the greatest increases in black performance due to the Flynn effect. This likely suggests that they are also the most sensitive to environmental changes, which undermines Jensen's argument that the black–white gap is most likely caused by genetic factors. Nisbett et al. (2012a, p. 146) make the same point, noting also that the increase in the IQ scores of black test takers necessarily indicates an increase in g.

James Flynn argued that his findings undermine Spearman's hypothesis. In a 2006 study, he and William Dickens found that between 1972 and 2002 "The standard measure of the g gap between Blacks and Whites declined virtually in tandem with the IQ gap." Flynn also criticized Jensen's basic assumption that a correlation between g-loading and test score gap implies a genetic cause for the gap. In a 2014 suite of meta-analyses, along with co-authors Jan te Nijenhuis and Daniel Metzen, he showed that the same negative correlation between IQ gains and g-loading obtains for cognitive deficits of known environmental cause: iodine deficiency, prenatal cocaine exposure, fetal alcohol syndrome, and traumatic brain injury.

Adoption studies

A number of IQ studies have been done on the effect of similar rearing conditions on children from different races. The hypothesis is that this can be determined by investigating whether black children adopted into white families demonstrated gains in IQ test scores relative to black children reared in black families. Depending on whether their test scores are more similar to their biological or adoptive families, that could be interpreted as supporting either a genetic or an environmental hypothesis. Critiques of such studies question whether the environment of black children—even when raised in white families—is truly comparable to the environment of white children. Several reviews of the adoption study literature have suggested that it is probably impossible to avoid confounding biological and environmental factors in this type of study. Another criticism by Nisbett et al. (2012a, pp. 134) is that adoption studies on the whole tend to be carried out in a restricted set of environments, mostly in the medium-high SES range, where heritability is higher than in the low-SES range.

The Minnesota Transracial Adoption Study (1976) examined the IQ test scores of 122 adopted children and 143 nonadopted children reared by advantaged white families. The children were restudied ten years later. The study found higher IQ for white people compared to black people, both at age 7 and age 17. Acknowledging the existence of confounding factors, Scarr and Weinberg, the authors of the original study, did not consider that it provided support for either the hereditarian or environmentalist view.

Three other studies lend support to environmental explanations of group IQ differences:

  • Eyferth (1961) studied the out-of-wedlock children of black and white soldiers stationed in Germany after World War II who were then raised by white German mothers in what has become known as the Eyferth study. He found no significant differences in average IQ between groups.
  • Tizard et al. (1972) studied black (West Indian), white, and mixed-race children raised in British long-stay residential nurseries. Two out of three tests found no significant differences. One test found higher scores for non-white people.
  • Moore (1986) compared black and mixed-race children adopted by either black or white middle-class families in the United States. Moore observed that 23 black and interracial children raised by white parents had a significantly higher mean score than 23 age-matched children raised by black parents (117 vs 104), and argued that differences in early socialization explained these differences.

Frydman and Lynn (1989) showed a mean IQ of 119 for Korean infants adopted by Belgian families. After correcting for the Flynn effect, the IQ of the adopted Korean children was still 10 points higher than that of the Belgian children.

Reviewing the evidence from adoption studies, Mackintosh finds that environmental and genetic variables remain confounded and considers evidence from adoption studies inconclusive, and fully compatible with a 100% environmental explanation. Similarly, Drew Thomas argues that race differences in IQ that appear in adoption studies are in fact an artifact of methodology, and that East Asian IQ advantages and black IQ disadvantages disappear when this is controlled for.

Racial admixture studies

Most people have ancestry from different geographical regions. In particular, African Americans typically have ancestors from both Africa and Europe, with, on average, 20% of their genome inherited from European ancestors. If racial IQ gaps have a partially genetic basis, one might expect black people with a higher degree of European ancestry to score higher on IQ tests than black people with less European ancestry, because the genes inherited from European ancestors would likely include some genes with a positive effect on IQ. Geneticist Alan Templeton has argued that an experiment based on the Mendelian "common garden" design, where specimens with different hybrid compositions are subjected to the same environmental influences, are the only way to definitively show a causal relation between genes and group differences in IQ. Summarizing the findings of admixture studies, he concludes that they have shown no significant correlation between any cognitive ability and the degree of African or European ancestry.

Studies have employed different ways of measuring or approximating relative degrees of ancestry from Africa and Europe. Some studies have used skin color as a measure, and others have used blood groups. Loehlin (2000) surveys the literature and argues that the blood groups studies may be seen as providing some support to the genetic hypothesis, even though the correlation between ancestry and IQ was quite low. He finds that studies by Eyferth (1961), Willerman, Naylor & Myrianthopoulos (1970) did not find a correlation between degree of African/European ancestry and IQ. The latter study did find a difference based on the race of the mother, with children of white mothers with black fathers scoring higher than children of black mothers and white fathers. Loehlin considers that such a finding is compatible with either a genetic or an environmental cause. All in all Loehlin finds admixture studies inconclusive and recommends more research.

Reviewing the evidence from admixture studies Hunt (2010) considers it to be inconclusive because of too many uncontrolled variables. Mackintosh (2011, p. 338) quotes a statement by Nisbett (2009) to the effect that admixture studies have not provided a shred of evidence in favor of a genetic basis for the IQ gap.

Mental chronometry

Main article: Mental chronometry

Mental chronometry measures the elapsed time between the presentation of a sensory stimulus and the subsequent behavioral response by the participant. These studies have shown inconsistent results when comparing black and white populations groups, with some studies showing whites outperforming blacks, and others showing blacks outperforming whites.

Arthur Jensen argued that this reaction time (RT) is a measure of the speed and efficiency with which the brain processes information, and that scores on most types of RT tasks tend to correlate with scores on standard IQ tests as well as with g. Nisbett argues that some studies have found correlations closer to 0.2, and that a correlation is not always found. Nisbett points to the Jensen & Whang (1993) study in which a group of Chinese Americans had longer reaction times than a group of European Americans, despite having higher IQs. Nisbett also mentions findings in Flynn (1991) and Deary (2001) suggesting that movement time (the measure of how long it takes a person to move a finger after making the decision to do so) correlates with IQ just as strongly as reaction time, and that average movement time is faster for black people than for white people. Mackintosh (2011, p. 339) considers reaction time evidence unconvincing and comments that other cognitive tests that also correlate well with IQ show no disparity at all, for example the habituation/dishabituation test. He further comments that studies show that rhesus monkeys have shorter reaction times than American college students, suggesting that different reaction times may not tell us anything useful about intelligence.

Brain size

Main article: Brain size

A number of studies have reported a moderate statistical correlation between differences in IQ and brain size between individuals in the same group. Some scholars have reported differences in average brain sizes between racial groups, although this is unlikely to be a good measure of IQ as brain size also differs between men and women, but without significant differences in IQ. At the same time newborn black children have the same average brain size as white children, suggesting that the difference in average size could be accounted for by differences in environment. Several environmental factors that reduce brain size have been demonstrated to disproportionately affect black children.

Archaeological data

Archaeological evidence does not support claims by Rushton and others that black people's cognitive ability was inferior to white people's during prehistoric times.

Policy relevance and ethics

Main article: Intelligence and public policy

The ethics of research on race and intelligence has long been a subject of debate: in a 1996 report of the American Psychological Association; in guidelines proposed by Gray and Thompson and by Hunt and Carlson; and in two editorials in Nature in 2009 by Steven Rose and by Stephen J. Ceci and Wendy M. Williams.

Steven Rose maintains that the history of eugenics makes this field of research difficult to reconcile with current ethical standards for science. On the other hand, James R. Flynn has argued that had there been a ban on research on possibly poorly conceived ideas, much valuable research on intelligence testing (including his own discovery of the Flynn effect) would not have occurred.

Many have argued for increased interventions in order to close the gaps. Flynn writes that "America will have to address all the aspects of black experience that are disadvantageous, beginning with the regeneration of inner city neighborhoods and their schools." Especially in developing nations, society has been urged to take on the prevention of cognitive impairment in children as a high priority. Possible preventable causes include malnutrition, infectious diseases such as meningitis, parasites, cerebral malaria, in utero drug and alcohol exposure, newborn asphyxia, low birth weight, head injuries, lead poisoning and endocrine disorders.

See also

References

Notes

Citations

  1. ^ Mercer, Jonathan (October 1, 2023). "Racism, Stereotypes, and War". direct.mit.edu. Journal of International Security. Retrieved 2024-02-04.
  2. Jackson & Weidman 2004, p. 23.
  3. Stewart, Roderick M. 1999. "The Claims of Frederick Douglass Philosophically Considered." Pp. 155–56 in Frederick Douglass: A Critical Reader, edited by B. E. Lawson and F. M. Kirkland. Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-631-20578-4. "Moreover, though he does not make the point explicitly, again the very fact that Douglass is ably disputing this argument on this occasion celebrating a select few's intellect and will (or moral character)—this fact constitutes a living counterexample to the narrowness of the pro-slavery definition of humans."
  4. Marable, Manning (2011), Living Black History: How Reimagining the African-American Past Can Remake America's Racial Future, p. 96. ISBN 978-0-465-04395-8.
  5. Tonooka, Chika (2017). "Reverse Emulation and the Cult of Japanese Efficiency in Edwardian Britain". The Historical Journal. 60 (1): 95–119. doi:10.1017/S0018246X15000539. ISSN 0018-246X. JSTOR 26343378. S2CID 162698331.
  6. Plotnik & Kouyoumdjian 2011.
  7. Terman, Lewis (1916). The Measurement Of Intelligence. Houghton, Mifflin and Company. p. 91. OCLC 557712625.
  8. Jackson & Weidman 2004, p. 116.
  9. Jackson & Weidman 2004, pp. 116, 309.
  10. Pickren & Rutherford 2010, p. 163.
  11. Jackson & Weidman 2004, p. 145.
  12. Spiro 2009.
  13. Ludy 2006
  14. Jackson & Weidman 2004, pp. 130–32.
  15. ^ Tucker 2002.
  16. Jackson 2005.
  17. Shurkin 2006.
  18. Jensen 1969, pp. 1–123.
  19. Panofsky, Aaron (2014). Misbehaving Science. Controversy and the Development of Behavior Genetics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0-226-05831-3.
  20. Alland 2002, pp. 79–80.
  21. Herrnstein & Murray 1994.
  22. ^ Maltby, Day & Macaskill 2007
  23. Mackintosh 1998
  24. Graves 2002a.
  25. Graves 2002b.
  26. Grossman & Kaufman 2001
  27. Adam, Miller (1994). "The Pioneer Fund: Bankrolling the Professors of Hate". The Journal of Blacks in Higher Education (6): 58–61. doi:10.2307/2962466. JSTOR 2962466.
  28. Blakemore, Bill; Jennings, Peter; Nissen, Beth (November 22, 1994). "The Bell Curve and the Pioneer Fund". ABC World News Tonight. ABC News. Archived from the original on March 3, 2016. Retrieved May 1, 2020. Vanderbilt Television News Archive : ABC Evening News for Tuesday, Nov 22, 1994. Headline: American Agenda (Intelligence) Archived January 3, 2016, at the Wayback Machine
  29. Montagu, Ashley (2002). Race and IQ (2 ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-510221-5.
  30. Berlet 2003.
  31. Pioneer Fund Board Archived 2011-05-25 at the Wayback Machine
  32. Falk 2008, p. 18
  33. Wroe 2008, p. 81
  34. ^ Schacter, Gilbert & Wegner 2007, pp. 350–1
  35. ^ Sternberg, Grigorenko & Kidd 2005
  36. Deary, I. J.; Lawn, M.; Bartholomew, D. J. (2008). ""A conversation between Charles Spearman, Godfrey Thomson, and Edward L. Thorndike: The International Examinations Inquiry Meetings 1931-1938": Correction to Deary, Lawn, and Bartholomew (2008)" (PDF). History of Psychology. 11 (3): 156–157. doi:10.1037/1093-4510.11.3.163. hdl:20.500.11820/5417f3c7-e873-40b9-ad73-19c6acc9e35b. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2020-08-06. Retrieved 2020-06-25.
  37. Jensen 1998, p. 445ff.
  38. Ceci, Stephen J. (1991). "How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence". Developmental Psychology. 27 (5): 703–722. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.27.5.703.
  39. ^ Dickens & Flynn 2006.
  40. Richie, Stuart J.; Tucker-Drob, Elliot (June 2018). "How Much Does Education Improve Intelligence? A Meta-Analysis". Psychological Science. 29 (8).
  41. Mackintosh 2011, p. 359.
  42. ^ Using Population Descriptors in Genetics and Genomics Research: A New Framework for an Evolving Field (Consensus Study Report). National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine. 2023. doi:10.17226/26902. ISBN 978-0-309-70065-8. PMID 36989389. In humans, race is a socially constructed designation, a misleading and harmful surrogate for population genetic differences, and has a long history of being incorrectly identified as the major genetic reason for phenotypic differences between groups.
  43. Daley & Onwuegbuzie 2011, p. 294.
  44. Templeton, A. (2016). EVOLUTION AND NOTIONS OF HUMAN RACE. In Losos J. & Lenski R. (Eds.), How Evolution Shapes Our Lives: Essays on Biology and Society (pp. 346–361). Princeton; Oxford: Princeton University Press. doi:10.2307/j.ctv7h0s6j.26. That this view reflects the consensus among American anthropologists is stated in: Wagner, Jennifer K.; Yu, Joon-Ho; Ifekwunigwe, Jayne O.; Harrell, Tanya M.; Bamshad, Michael J.; Royal, Charmaine D. (February 2017). "Anthropologists' views on race, ancestry, and genetics". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 162 (2): 318–327. doi:10.1002/ajpa.23120. PMC 5299519. PMID 27874171. See also: American Association of Physical Anthropologists (27 March 2019). "AAPA Statement on Race and Racism". American Association of Physical Anthropologists. Archived from the original on 25 January 2022. Retrieved 19 June 2020.
  45. Smay & Armelagos 2000.
  46. Rotimi, Charles N. (2004). "Are medical and nonmedical uses of large-scale genomic markers conflating genetics and 'race'?". Nature Genetics. 36 (11 Suppl): 43–47. doi:10.1038/ng1439. PMID 15508002. Two facts are relevant: (i) as a result of different evolutionary forces, including natural selection, there are geographical patterns of genetic variations that correspond, for the most part, to continental origin; and (ii) observed patterns of geographical differences in genetic information do not correspond to our notion of social identities, including 'race' and 'ethnicity
  47. Schaefer 2008
  48. AAA 1998
  49. "AAPA Statement on Race & Racism". Archived from the original on 2022-01-25. Retrieved 2020-06-28.
  50. Brace 2005
  51. Kaplan, Jonathan Michael; Winther, Rasmus Grønfeldt (2014). "Realism, Antirealism, and Conventionalism About Race". Philosophy of Science. 81 (5): 1039–1052. doi:10.1086/678314. S2CID 55148854.
  52. Winther, Rasmus Grønfeldt (2015). "The Genetic Reification of 'Race'?: A Story of Two Mathematical Methods" (PDF). Critical Philosophy of Race. 2 (2): 204–223.
  53. Kaplan & Winther (2013).
  54. Graves, Joseph (7 June 2006). "What We Know and What We Don't Know: Human Genetic Variation and the Social Construction of Race". Race and Genomics. Archived from the original on 3 June 2019. Retrieved 3 December 2023.
  55. Weiss, K. M.; Fullerton, S. M. (2005). "Racing around, getting nowhere". Evolutionary Anthropology. 14 (5): 165–169. doi:10.1002/evan.20079. S2CID 84927946.
  56. Mills, C. W. (1988). "But What Are You Really? The Metaphysics of Race". Blackness visible: essays on philosophy and race. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press. pp. 41–66.
  57. Rowe 2005
  58. ^ Hunt & Carlson 2007
  59. Sternberg & Grigorenko 2007
  60. ^ Neisser et al. 1996.
  61. ^ Jackson & Weidman 2004, p. 222.
  62. Hampshire et al. 2012.
  63. Reynolds, Cecil R.; Altmann, Robert A.; Allen, Daniel N. (2021). "The Problem of Bias in Psychological Assessment". Mastering Modern Psychological Testing. Springer. pp. 573–613, 582. doi:10.1007/978-3-030-59455-8_15. ISBN 978-3-030-59454-1. S2CID 236660997.
  64. Encyclopedia of Diversity in Education. SAGE. 2012. p. 1209. ISBN 978-1-4129-8152-1. Archived from the original on 2023-03-20. Retrieved 2018-01-20.
  65. Roth et al. 2001
  66. Demie, Feyisa; McLean, Christabel (1 December 2007). "Raising the achievement of African heritage pupils: a case study of good practice in British schools". Educational Studies. 33 (4): 415–434. doi:10.1080/03055690701423606. ISSN 0305-5698. S2CID 144579288.
  67. Rutter, Jill (2013). Back to basics: Towards a successful and cost-effective integration policy. Institute for Public Policy Research. p. 43. Archived from the original on 2020-04-13. Retrieved 2020-05-23. {{cite book}}: |work= ignored (help)
  68. Feyisa Demie, Andrew Hau (2016). Language Diversity and Attainment in Primary Schools in England (PDF). Lambeth Research And Statistics Unit. p. 18. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2020-08-06. Retrieved 2020-05-24.
  69. "GCSE English and maths results". Gov.UK. 2019. Archived from the original on 2022-09-20. Retrieved 2022-09-20.
  70. Mackintosh 1998, p. 162
  71. Swain, Carol (2003). Contemporary voices of white nationalism in America. Cambridge, UK New York: Cambridge University Press. p. 70. ISBN 978-0-521-01693-3. Note: this quote is from the authors' introductory essay, not from the interviews.
  72. Dickens & Flynn 2001.
  73. Vincent 1991.
  74. Neisser, Ulric (Ed). 1998. The rising curve: Long-term gains in IQ and related measures. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association
  75. Mackintosh 2011.
  76. ^ Nisbett et al. 2012a.
  77. ^ Bellinger, Stiles & Needleman 1992
  78. MMWR 2005
  79. ^ Campbell et al. 2002
  80. Ivanovic et al. 2004
  81. Saloojee & Pettifor 2001
  82. Agency For Toxic Substances And Disease Registry Case Studies In Environmental Medicine (CSEM) (2012-02-15). "Principles of Pediatric Environmental Health, The Child as Susceptible Host: A Developmental Approach to Pediatric Environmental Medicine" (PDF). U.S. Department for Health and Human Services. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2019-01-31. Retrieved 2019-01-30.
  83. Lanphear, Bruce P.; Hornung, Richard; Khoury, Jane; Yolton, Kimberly; Baghurst, Peter; Bellinger, David C.; Canfield, Richard L.; Dietrich, Kim N.; Bornschein, Robert; Greene, Tom; Rothenberg, Stephen J.; Needleman, Herbert L.; Schnaas, Lourdes; Wasserman, Gail; Graziano, Joseph; Roberts, Russell (2005-03-18). "Low-Level Environmental Lead Exposure and Children's Intellectual Function: An International Pooled Analysis". Environmental Health Perspectives. 113 (7): 894–899. Bibcode:2005EnvHP.113..894L. doi:10.1289/ehp.7688. ISSN 0091-6765. PMC 1257652. PMID 16002379.
  84. Qian et al. 2005
  85. Feyrer, James; Politi, Dimitra; Weil, David N. (2017). "The Cognitive Effects of Micronutrient Deficiency: Evidence from Salt Iodization in the United States" (PDF). Journal of the European Economic Association. 15 (2): 355–387. doi:10.1093/jeea/jvw002. PMC 6919660. PMID 31853231. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2020-08-13. Retrieved 2019-07-22.
  86. The Lancet Series on Maternal and Child Undernutrition Archived 2011-07-17 at the Wayback Machine, 2008.
  87. Mackintosh 2011, pp. 343–44.
  88. Behrman, Alderman & Hoddinott 2004
  89. Colom, R.; Lluis-Font, J. M.; Andrés-Pueyo, A. (2005). "The generational intelligence gains are caused by decreasing variance in the lower half of the distribution: supporting evidence for the nutrition hypothesis". Intelligence. 33: 83–91. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.010.
  90. Flynn, J. R. (2009a). "Requiem for nutrition as the cause of IQ gains: Raven's gains in Britain 1938 to 2008". Economics and Human Biology. 7 (1): 18–27. doi:10.1016/j.ehb.2009.01.009. PMID 19251490.
  91. Eppig 2011
  92. Horta, Bernardo L.; Victoria, Cesar G. (2013). "Long-term effects of breastfeeding – a systemic review" (PDF). World Health Organization. Archived (PDF) from the original on 9 April 2020. Retrieved 18 June 2018.
  93. Colen, Cynthia G.; Ramey, David M. (2014). "Is Breast Truly Best? Estimating the Effect of Breastfeeding on Long-term Child Wellbeing in the United States Using Sibling Comparisons". Social Science & Medicine. 109 (1): 55–65. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.01.027. PMC 4077166. PMID 24698713.
  94. Reichman 2005
  95. Manly et al. 2002 and Manly et al. 2004
  96. Mickelson 2003
  97. Elhoweris et al. 2005
  98. Miele 2002, p. 133
  99. Fagan, Joseph F; Holland, Cynthia R (2002). "Equal opportunity and racial differences in IQ". Intelligence. 30 (4): 361–387. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(02)00080-6.
  100. Fagan, J.F.; Holland, C.R. (2007). "Racial equality in intelligence: Predictions from a theory of intelligence as processing". Intelligence. 35 (4): 319–334. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2006.08.009.
  101. Daley & Onwuegbuzie 2011.
  102. Marks, D.F. (2010). "IQ variations across time, race, and nationality: An artifact of differences in literacy skills". Psychological Reports. 106 (3): 643–664. doi:10.2466/pr0.106.3.643-664. PMID 20712152. S2CID 12179547.
  103. Barry, Scott (2010-08-23). "The Flynn Effect and IQ Disparities Among Races, Ethnicities, and Nations: Are There Common Links?". Psychology Today. Archived from the original on 2023-03-20. Retrieved 2014-08-22.
  104. McKay, Patrick F.; Doverspike, Dennis; Bowen-Hilton, Doreen; McKay, Quintonia D. (2003). "The Effects of Demographic Variables and Stereotype Threat on Black/White Differences in Cognitive Ability Test Performance". Journal of Business and Psychology. 18 (1): 1–14. doi:10.1023/A:1025062703113. S2CID 142317051.
  105. Hunt 2010, p. 428.
  106. Magnuson, Katherine A.; Duncan, Greg J. (December 2006). "The role of family socioeconomic resources in the black–white test score gap among young children". Developmental Review. 26 (4): 365–399. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2006.06.004.
  107. Scarr-Salapatek, S. (1971). "Race, social class, and IQ". Science. 174 (4016): 1285–95. Bibcode:1971Sci...174.1285S. doi:10.1126/science.174.4016.1285. PMID 5167501.
  108. Scarr-Salapatek, S. (1974). "Some myths about heritability and IQ". Nature. 251 (5475): 463–464. Bibcode:1974Natur.251..463S. doi:10.1038/251463b0. S2CID 32437709.
  109. D. C. Rowe. (1994). The Limits of Family Influence: Genes, Experience and Behaviour. Guilford Press. London
  110. Kirkpatrick, R. M.; McGue, M.; Iacono, W. G. (2015). "Replication of a gene-environment interaction Via Multimodel inference: additive-genetic variance in adolescents' general cognitive ability increases with family-of-origin socioeconomic status". Behav Genet. 45 (2): 200–14. doi:10.1007/s10519-014-9698-y. PMC 4374354. PMID 25539975.
  111. Bronfenbrenner, Urie; Ceci, Stephen J. (October 1994). "Nature-nuture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model". Psychological Review. 101 (4): 568–586. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.101.4.568. PMID 7984707. S2CID 17402964.
  112. Cottrell, Newman & Roisman 2015.
  113. Cronshaw et al. 2006, p. 278
  114. Verney et al. 2005
  115. Borsboom 2006
  116. Shuttleworth-Edwards et al. 2004
  117. Richardson 2004
  118. Hunt & Wittmann 2008
  119. Irvine 1983
  120. Irvine & Berry 1988 a collection of articles by several authors discussing the limits of assessment by intelligence tests in different communities in the world. In particular, Reuning (1988) describes the difficulties in devising and administering tests for Kalahari bushmen.
  121. Mackintosh 1998, p. 174: "Despite widespread belief to the contrary, however, there is ample evidence, both in Britain and the USA, that IQ tests predict educational attainment just about as well in ethnic minorities as in the white majority."
  122. "Reflections about intelligence over 40 years" (PDF). Intelligence (70): 73–83. 2018. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2019-02-03. Retrieved 2019-02-02.
  123. Aronson, Wilson & Akert 2005
  124. Steele, Claude M. (1997). "A threat in the air: How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance". American Psychologist. 52 (6): 613–629. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.319.8283. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.6.613. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 9174398. S2CID 19952.
  125. Mackintosh 2011, p. 348.
  126. Ogbu 1978.
  127. Ogbu 1994.
  128. Chan, D.; Schmitt, N.; DeShon, R. P.; Clause, C. S.; Delbridge, K. (April 1997). "Reactions to cognitive ability tests: the relationships between race, test performance, face validity perceptions, and test-taking motivation". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 82 (2): 300–310. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.82.2.300. ISSN 0021-9010. PMID 9109288.
  129. Ganley CM, Mingle LA, Ryan AM, Ryan K, Vasilyeva M, Perry M (October 2013). "An examination of stereotype threat effects on girls' mathematics performance" (PDF). Developmental Psychology. 49 (10): 1886–97. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.353.4436. doi:10.1037/a0031412. PMID 23356523. Archived from the original (PDF) on 19 July 2014.
  130. Stoet G, Geary DC (2012). "Can stereotype threat explain the gender gap in mathematics performance and achievement?". Review of General Psychology. 16: 93–102. doi:10.1037/a0026617. S2CID 145724069. Pdf. Archived 2016-01-12 at the Wayback Machine
  131. Flore PC, Wicherts JM (February 2015). "Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis". Journal of School Psychology. 53 (1): 25–44. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002. PMID 25636259. S2CID 206516995.
  132. Sackett PR, Hardison CM, Cullen MJ (January 2004). "On interpreting stereotype threat as accounting for African American-White differences on cognitive tests" (PDF). The American Psychologist. 59 (1): 7–13. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59.1.7. PMID 14736315. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-04-04.
  133. Pennington CR, Heim D, Levy AR, Larkin DT (2016-01-11). "Twenty Years of Stereotype Threat Research: A Review of Psychological Mediators". PLOS ONE. 11 (1): e0146487. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1146487P. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0146487. PMC 4713435. PMID 26752551.
  134. ^ Nguyen HH, Ryan AM (November 2008). "Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 93 (6): 1314–34. doi:10.1037/a0012702. PMID 19025250. S2CID 36769821.
  135. ^ Walton, Gregory M.; Spencer, Steven J. (2009-09-01). "Latent Ability: Grades and Test Scores Systematically Underestimate the Intellectual Ability of Negatively Stereotyped Students". Psychological Science. 20 (9): 1132–1139. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02417.x. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 19656335. S2CID 25810191.
  136. Gentile, Ambra; Boca, Stefano; Giammusso, Isabella (2018-11-01). "'You play like a Woman!' Effects of gender stereotype threat on Women's performance in physical and sport activities: A meta-analysis". Psychology of Sport and Exercise. 39: 95–103. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.07.013. ISSN 1469-0292. S2CID 149490634.
  137. Lamont, Ruth A.; Swift, Hannah J.; Abrams, Dominic (2015). "A Review and Meta-Analysis of Age-Based Stereotype Threat: Negative Stereotypes, Not Facts, Do the Damage". Psychology and Aging. 30 (1): 180–193. doi:10.1037/a0038586. ISSN 1939-1498. PMC 4360754. PMID 25621742.
  138. Picho, Katherine; Rodriguez, Ariel; Finnie, Lauren (May 2013). "Exploring the Moderating Role of Context on the Mathematics Performance of Females Under Stereotype Threat: A Meta-Analysis". The Journal of Social Psychology. 153 (3): 299–333. doi:10.1080/00224545.2012.737380. PMID 23724702. S2CID 45950675.
  139. Liu, Songqi; Liu, Pei; Wang, Mo; Zhang, Baoshan (July 2020). "Effectiveness of Stereotype Threat Interventions: A Meta-Analytic Review". Journal of Applied Psychology. currently in press (6): 921–949. doi:10.1037/apl0000770. PMID 32772526. S2CID 221098319.
  140. Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (2012). "Intelligence: New findings and theoretical developments". American Psychologist. 67 (2): 130–159. doi:10.1037/a0026699. ISSN 1935-990X. PMID 22233090.
  141. ^ Nisbett, Richard E.; Aronson, Joshua; Blair, Clancy; Dickens, William; Flynn, James; Halpern, Diane F.; Turkheimer, Eric (2012). "Group differences in IQ are best understood as environmental in origin". American Psychologist. 67 (6): 503–504. doi:10.1037/a0029772. ISSN 0003-066X. PMID 22963427.
  142. Bird, Kevin; Jackson, John P.; Winston, Andrew S. (2024). "Confronting Scientific Racism in Psychology: Lessons from Evolutionary Biology and Genetics". American Psychologist. 79 (4): 497–508. doi:10.1037/amp0001228. PMID 39037836. Recent articles claim that the folk categories of race are genetically meaningful divisions, and that evolved genetic differences among races and nations are important for explaining immutable differences in cognitive ability, educational attainment, crime, sexual behavior, and wealth; all claims that are opposed by a strong scientific consensus to the contrary.
  143. Ceci & Williams 2009, pp. 788–789, "There is an emerging consensus about racial and gender equality in genetic determinants of intelligence, most researchers, including ourselves, agree that genes do not explain between-group differences".
  144. Hunt 2010, p. 447, "It is worth remembering that no genes related to difference in cognitive skills across the various racial and ethnic groups have ever been discovered. The argument for genetic differences has been carried forward largely by circumstantial evidence. Of course, tomorrow afternoon genetic mechanisms producing racial and ethnic differences in intelligence might be discovered, but there have been a lot of investigations, and tomorrow has not come for quite some time now."
  145. Mackintosh 2011, pp. 334–338, 344.
  146. ^ Kaplan, Jonathan Michael (January 2015). "Race, IQ, and the search for statistical signals associated with so-called "X"-factors: environments, racism, and the "hereditarian hypothesis"". Biology & Philosophy. 30 (1): 1–17. doi:10.1007/s10539-014-9428-0. ISSN 0169-3867. S2CID 85351431.
  147. Panofsky, Aaron; Dasgupta, Kushan; Iturriaga, Nicole (2021). "How White nationalists mobilize genetics: From genetic ancestry and human biodiversity to counterscience and metapolitics". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 175 (2): 387–398. doi:10.1002/ajpa.24150. ISSN 0002-9483. PMC 9909835. PMID 32986847. he claims that genetics defines racial groups and makes them different, that IQ and cultural differences among racial groups are caused by genes, and that racial inequalities within and between nations are the inevitable outcome of long evolutionary processes are neither new nor supported by science (either old or new).
  148. Lewontin, Richard C. (March 1970). "Race and Intelligence". Bulletin of the Atomic Scientists. 26 (3): 2–8. Bibcode:1970BuAtS..26c...2L. doi:10.1080/00963402.1970.11457774. Archived from the original on 10 June 2021. Retrieved 26 April 2021.
  149. Nevid 2014, p. 271.
  150. ^ Templeton 2001
  151. ^ "Intelligence research should not be held back by its past". Nature. 545 (7655): 385–386. 25 May 2017. Bibcode:2017Natur.545R.385.. doi:10.1038/nature.2017.22021. PMID 28541341. S2CID 4449918.
  152. Bird, Kevin A. (2 February 2021). "No support for the hereditarian hypothesis of the Black–White achievement gap using polygenic scores and tests for divergent selection". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 175 (2): 465–476. doi:10.1002/ajpa.24216. ISSN 0002-9483. PMID 33529393. Retrieved 1 November 2024 – via Wiley Online Library.
  153. Carson, Michael; Beckwith, Jon (2001). 'Race', IQ and Genes. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. pp. 1–5. doi:10.1002/9780470015902.a0005689.pub3. ISBN 978-0-470-01590-2.
  154. Beckwith, Jon; Morris, Corey A. (December 2008). "Twin Studies of Political Behavior: Untenable Assumptions?". Perspectives on Politics. 6 (4): 785–791. doi:10.1017/S1537592708081917. ISSN 1541-0986. S2CID 55630117.
  155. Kamin, Leon J.; Goldberger, Arthur S. (February 2002). "Twin Studies in Behavioral Research: A Skeptical View". Theoretical Population Biology. 61 (1): 83–95. Bibcode:2002TPBio..61...83K. doi:10.1006/tpbi.2001.1555. ISSN 0040-5809. PMID 11895384.
  156. Moore & Shenk 2016.
  157. Mackenzie 1984
  158. Deary, Johnson & Houlihan 2009
  159. ^ Block 2002
  160. Rushton & Jensen 2005.
  161. Flynn, J.R. (1999). "Searching for justice: the discovery of IQ gains over time" (PDF). American Psychologist. 54: 5–9. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.1.5. Archived (PDF) from the original on 25 June 2010. Retrieved 26 October 2017.
  162. Flynn, James R. (2010). "The spectacles through which I see the race and IQ debate" (PDF). Intelligence. 38 (4): 363–366. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.05.001. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2020-12-07. Retrieved 2011-02-18.
  163. Flynn, James R.; te Nijenhuis, Jan; Metzen, Daniel (2014). "The g beyond Spearman's g: Flynn's paradoxes resolved using four exploratory meta-analyses" (PDF). Intelligence. 44: 1–10. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2014.01.009.
  164. ^ Mackintosh 2011, p. 337.
  165. ^ Weinberg, Scarr & Waldman 1992
  166. Scarr & Weinberg 1976.
  167. Loehlin 2000, p. 185.
  168. Scarr & Weinberg 1990.
  169. Loehlin 2000, p. 187.
  170. Frydman and Lynn (1989). "The intelligence of Korean children adopted in Belgium". Personality and Individual Differences. 10 (12): 1323–1325. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(89)90246-8.
  171. Thomas, Drew (2017). "Racial IQ Differences among Transracial Adoptees: Fact or Artifact?". Journal of Intelligence. 5 (1): 1. doi:10.3390/jintelligence5010001. PMC 6526420. PMID 31162392.
  172. Bryc et al. 2009
  173. Loehlin 2000.
  174. Templeton 2001.
  175. Sheppard & Vernon 2008.
  176. ^ Jensen 2006
  177. Nisbett 2009
  178. Nisbett 2009, pp. 221–2.
  179. Deary, Penke & Johnson 2010.
  180. McDaniel 2005.
  181. Ho et al. 1980.
  182. MacEachern 2006.
  183. Gray & Thompson 2004
  184. Ceci & Williams 2009
  185. ^ Rose, Steven (2009). "Should scientists study race and IQ? NO: Science and society do not benefit". Nature. 457 (7231): 786–788. Bibcode:2009Natur.457..786R. doi:10.1038/457786a. PMID 19212384. S2CID 42846614.
  186. Flynn 2009b
  187. Jencks, Christopher; Phillips, Meredith. "The Black-White Test Score Gap". New York Times. Archived from the original on 8 October 2016. Retrieved 2 October 2016.
  188. Flynn 2008
  189. Olness 2003

Bibliography

Human intelligence topics
Types
Abilities, traits,
and constructs
Models and theories
Areas of research
Categories:
Race and intelligence: Difference between revisions Add topic